The role of neuronal excitability, allocation to an engram and memory linking in the behavioral generation of a false memory in mice
[Display omitted] •Previous research shows memory for 2 events experienced closely in time may be linked due to neuronal excitability and co-allocation of engrams.•Here, we show this process may be hijacked to form a false memory in mice.•False fear memory created if neutral CS presented within hour...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Neurobiology of learning and memory 2020-10, Vol.174, p.107284-107284, Article 107284 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
Zusammenfassung: | [Display omitted]
•Previous research shows memory for 2 events experienced closely in time may be linked due to neuronal excitability and co-allocation of engrams.•Here, we show this process may be hijacked to form a false memory in mice.•False fear memory created if neutral CS presented within hours after fear conditioning.•False fear memory not created if neutral CS presented longer after fear conditioning.•Optogenetic investigation showed false memory was due to engram excitability post-training.
Memory is a constructive, not reproductive, process that is prone to errors. Errors in memory, though, may originate from normally adaptive memory processes. At the extreme of memory distortion is falsely “remembering” an event that did not occur. False memories are well-studied in cognitive psychology, but have received relatively less attention in neuroscience. Here, we took advantage of mechanistic insights into how neurons are allocated or recruited into an engram (memory trace) to generate a false memory in mice using only behavioral manipulations. At the time of an event, neurons compete for allocation to an engram supporting the memory for this event; neurons with higher excitability win this competition (Han et al., 2007). Even after the event, these allocated “engram neurons” remain temporarily (~6 h) more excitable than neighboring neurons. Should a similar event occur in this 6 h period of heightened engram neuron excitability, an overlapping population of neurons will be co-allocated to this second engram, which serves to functionally link the two memories (Rashid et al., 2016). Here, we applied this principle of co-allocation and found that mice develop a false fear memory to a neutral stimulus if exposed to this stimulus shortly (3 h), but not a longer time (24 h), after cued fear conditioning. Similar to co-allocation, the generation of this false memory depended on the post-training excitability of engram neurons such that these neurons remained more excitable during exposure to the neutral stimulus at 3 h but not 24 h. Optogenetically silencing engram neurons 3 h after cued fear conditioning impaired formation of a false fear memory to the neutral stimulus, while optogenetically activating engram neurons 24 h after cued fear conditioning created a false fear memory. These results suggest that some false memories may originate from normally adaptive mnemonic processes such as neuronal excitability-dependent allocation and memory linking. |
---|---|
ISSN: | 1074-7427 1095-9564 |
DOI: | 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107284 |