Systematic mappings of sound to meaning: A theoretical review
The form of a word sometimes conveys semantic information. For example, the iconic word gurgle sounds like what it means, and busy is easy to identify as an English adjective because it ends in - y . Such links between form and meaning matter because they help people learn and use language. But gurg...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Psychonomic bulletin & review 2024-04, Vol.31 (2), p.627-648 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
Zusammenfassung: | The form of a word sometimes conveys semantic information. For example, the iconic word
gurgle
sounds like what it means, and
busy
is easy to identify as an English adjective because it ends in -
y
. Such links between form and meaning matter because they help people learn and use language. But
gurgle
also sounds like
gargle
and
burble
, and the -
y
in
busy
is morphologically and etymologically unrelated to the -
y
in
crazy
and
watery
. Whatever processing effects
gurgle
and
busy
have in common likely stem not from iconic, morphological, or etymological relationships but from systematicity more broadly: the phenomenon whereby semantically related words share a phonological or orthographic feature. In this review, we evaluate corpus evidence that spoken languages are systematic (even when controlling for iconicity, morphology, and etymology) and experimental evidence that systematicity impacts word processing (even in lieu of iconic, morphological, and etymological relationships). We conclude by drawing attention to the relationship between systematicity and low-frequency words and, consequently, the role that systematicity plays in natural language processing. |
---|---|
ISSN: | 1069-9384 1531-5320 1531-5320 |
DOI: | 10.3758/s13423-023-02395-y |