Which measures of time preference best predict outcomes: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment

► A variety of different methods have been developed to measure time preferences. ► It is unclear which measures best predict outcomes. ► A large sample of non-students is surveyed using many of the standard methods. ► The (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best....

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of economic behavior & organization 2012-09, Vol.84 (1), p.308-320
Hauptverfasser: Burks, Stephen, Carpenter, Jeffrey, Götte, Lorenz, Rustichini, Aldo
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 320
container_issue 1
container_start_page 308
container_title Journal of economic behavior & organization
container_volume 84
creator Burks, Stephen
Carpenter, Jeffrey
Götte, Lorenz
Rustichini, Aldo
description ► A variety of different methods have been developed to measure time preferences. ► It is unclear which measures best predict outcomes. ► A large sample of non-students is surveyed using many of the standard methods. ► The (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best. Economists and psychologists have devised numerous instruments to measure time preferences and have generated a rich literature examining the extent to which time preferences predict important outcomes; however, we still do not know which measures work best. With the help of a large sample of non-student participants and administrative data on outcomes, we gather four different time preference measures and test the extent to which they predict both on their own and when they are all forced to compete head-to-head. Our results suggest that the now familiar (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best, especially when both parameters are used.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.03.012
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1373491597</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0167268112000674</els_id><sourcerecordid>1373491597</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c601t-ba8d159c09dfa34a179dde9a00c5c38950954aa8b4a938edec085bf4b16405e83</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkUFv3CAQhVHUSNkm-QM5IeXSi93B2AaiXqooSStF6qVRjgjDOItlL1uwV82_D8721EPL5Qn4ZmDeI-SKQcmAtZ-HcsAulBWwqgReZjkhGyaFKpho2AeyyZAoqlayM_IxpQHyEpXaEHzeerulE5q0REw09HT2E9J9xB4j7izSDtO87p23Mw3LbMOE6YbeHbx7v-9jmKiho4kvWCRrxnzkcXQUf-8x5ma7-YKc9mZMePlHz8nT_d3P22_F44-H77dfHwvbApuLzkjHGmVBud7w2jChnENlAGxjuVQNqKY2Rna1UVyiQwuy6fq6Y20NDUp-Tj4d--5j-LXkf-vJJ4vjaHYYlqQZF7xW-Qnxf7RqBVRSAGT0-i90CEvc5UE0YwIa1UrBM1UdKRtDStk-vc_Dm_iqGeg1JD3oNSS9hqSB6yy56MuxCLMrB49RJ-tXV52PaGftgv9X-RtE4Zp7</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1170596873</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Which measures of time preference best predict outcomes: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Burks, Stephen ; Carpenter, Jeffrey ; Götte, Lorenz ; Rustichini, Aldo</creator><creatorcontrib>Burks, Stephen ; Carpenter, Jeffrey ; Götte, Lorenz ; Rustichini, Aldo</creatorcontrib><description>► A variety of different methods have been developed to measure time preferences. ► It is unclear which measures best predict outcomes. ► A large sample of non-students is surveyed using many of the standard methods. ► The (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best. Economists and psychologists have devised numerous instruments to measure time preferences and have generated a rich literature examining the extent to which time preferences predict important outcomes; however, we still do not know which measures work best. With the help of a large sample of non-student participants and administrative data on outcomes, we gather four different time preference measures and test the extent to which they predict both on their own and when they are all forced to compete head-to-head. Our results suggest that the now familiar (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best, especially when both parameters are used.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0167-2681</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1879-1751</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.03.012</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JEBOD9</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Bias ; Consumption ; Discounting ; Economic behaviour ; Economic conditions ; Economic organization ; Economic theory ; Economists ; Field experiment ; Field work ; Impatience ; Measurement ; Parameters ; Preferences ; Present bias ; Psychologists ; Studies ; Time preference</subject><ispartof>Journal of economic behavior &amp; organization, 2012-09, Vol.84 (1), p.308-320</ispartof><rights>2012 Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>Copyright Elsevier Sequoia S.A. Sep 2012</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c601t-ba8d159c09dfa34a179dde9a00c5c38950954aa8b4a938edec085bf4b16405e83</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c601t-ba8d159c09dfa34a179dde9a00c5c38950954aa8b4a938edec085bf4b16405e83</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0167268112000674$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,27903,27904,30978,30979,65309</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Burks, Stephen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Carpenter, Jeffrey</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Götte, Lorenz</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rustichini, Aldo</creatorcontrib><title>Which measures of time preference best predict outcomes: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment</title><title>Journal of economic behavior &amp; organization</title><description>► A variety of different methods have been developed to measure time preferences. ► It is unclear which measures best predict outcomes. ► A large sample of non-students is surveyed using many of the standard methods. ► The (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best. Economists and psychologists have devised numerous instruments to measure time preferences and have generated a rich literature examining the extent to which time preferences predict important outcomes; however, we still do not know which measures work best. With the help of a large sample of non-student participants and administrative data on outcomes, we gather four different time preference measures and test the extent to which they predict both on their own and when they are all forced to compete head-to-head. Our results suggest that the now familiar (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best, especially when both parameters are used.</description><subject>Bias</subject><subject>Consumption</subject><subject>Discounting</subject><subject>Economic behaviour</subject><subject>Economic conditions</subject><subject>Economic organization</subject><subject>Economic theory</subject><subject>Economists</subject><subject>Field experiment</subject><subject>Field work</subject><subject>Impatience</subject><subject>Measurement</subject><subject>Parameters</subject><subject>Preferences</subject><subject>Present bias</subject><subject>Psychologists</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Time preference</subject><issn>0167-2681</issn><issn>1879-1751</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2012</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkUFv3CAQhVHUSNkm-QM5IeXSi93B2AaiXqooSStF6qVRjgjDOItlL1uwV82_D8721EPL5Qn4ZmDeI-SKQcmAtZ-HcsAulBWwqgReZjkhGyaFKpho2AeyyZAoqlayM_IxpQHyEpXaEHzeerulE5q0REw09HT2E9J9xB4j7izSDtO87p23Mw3LbMOE6YbeHbx7v-9jmKiho4kvWCRrxnzkcXQUf-8x5ma7-YKc9mZMePlHz8nT_d3P22_F44-H77dfHwvbApuLzkjHGmVBud7w2jChnENlAGxjuVQNqKY2Rna1UVyiQwuy6fq6Y20NDUp-Tj4d--5j-LXkf-vJJ4vjaHYYlqQZF7xW-Qnxf7RqBVRSAGT0-i90CEvc5UE0YwIa1UrBM1UdKRtDStk-vc_Dm_iqGeg1JD3oNSS9hqSB6yy56MuxCLMrB49RJ-tXV52PaGftgv9X-RtE4Zp7</recordid><startdate>20120901</startdate><enddate>20120901</enddate><creator>Burks, Stephen</creator><creator>Carpenter, Jeffrey</creator><creator>Götte, Lorenz</creator><creator>Rustichini, Aldo</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Sequoia S.A</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20120901</creationdate><title>Which measures of time preference best predict outcomes: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment</title><author>Burks, Stephen ; Carpenter, Jeffrey ; Götte, Lorenz ; Rustichini, Aldo</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c601t-ba8d159c09dfa34a179dde9a00c5c38950954aa8b4a938edec085bf4b16405e83</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2012</creationdate><topic>Bias</topic><topic>Consumption</topic><topic>Discounting</topic><topic>Economic behaviour</topic><topic>Economic conditions</topic><topic>Economic organization</topic><topic>Economic theory</topic><topic>Economists</topic><topic>Field experiment</topic><topic>Field work</topic><topic>Impatience</topic><topic>Measurement</topic><topic>Parameters</topic><topic>Preferences</topic><topic>Present bias</topic><topic>Psychologists</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Time preference</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Burks, Stephen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Carpenter, Jeffrey</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Götte, Lorenz</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rustichini, Aldo</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><jtitle>Journal of economic behavior &amp; organization</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Burks, Stephen</au><au>Carpenter, Jeffrey</au><au>Götte, Lorenz</au><au>Rustichini, Aldo</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Which measures of time preference best predict outcomes: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment</atitle><jtitle>Journal of economic behavior &amp; organization</jtitle><date>2012-09-01</date><risdate>2012</risdate><volume>84</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>308</spage><epage>320</epage><pages>308-320</pages><issn>0167-2681</issn><eissn>1879-1751</eissn><coden>JEBOD9</coden><abstract>► A variety of different methods have been developed to measure time preferences. ► It is unclear which measures best predict outcomes. ► A large sample of non-students is surveyed using many of the standard methods. ► The (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best. Economists and psychologists have devised numerous instruments to measure time preferences and have generated a rich literature examining the extent to which time preferences predict important outcomes; however, we still do not know which measures work best. With the help of a large sample of non-student participants and administrative data on outcomes, we gather four different time preference measures and test the extent to which they predict both on their own and when they are all forced to compete head-to-head. Our results suggest that the now familiar (β, δ) formulation of present bias and exponential discounting predicts best, especially when both parameters are used.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><doi>10.1016/j.jebo.2012.03.012</doi><tpages>13</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0167-2681
ispartof Journal of economic behavior & organization, 2012-09, Vol.84 (1), p.308-320
issn 0167-2681
1879-1751
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_1373491597
source Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Bias
Consumption
Discounting
Economic behaviour
Economic conditions
Economic organization
Economic theory
Economists
Field experiment
Field work
Impatience
Measurement
Parameters
Preferences
Present bias
Psychologists
Studies
Time preference
title Which measures of time preference best predict outcomes: Evidence from a large-scale field experiment
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-27T23%3A43%3A04IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Which%20measures%20of%20time%20preference%20best%20predict%20outcomes:%20Evidence%20from%20a%20large-scale%20field%20experiment&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20economic%20behavior%20&%20organization&rft.au=Burks,%20Stephen&rft.date=2012-09-01&rft.volume=84&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=308&rft.epage=320&rft.pages=308-320&rft.issn=0167-2681&rft.eissn=1879-1751&rft.coden=JEBOD9&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.jebo.2012.03.012&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1373491597%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1170596873&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0167268112000674&rfr_iscdi=true