Attentional control transfers beyond the reference frame
Much research has shown that humans can allocate attentional control differentially to multiple locations based on the amount of conflict historically associated with a given location. Additionally, once established, these control settings can transfer to nearby locations that themselves have no con...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Psychological research 2020-02, Vol.84 (1), p.217-230 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 230 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 217 |
container_title | Psychological research |
container_volume | 84 |
creator | Weidler, Blaire J. Dey, Abhishek Bugg, Julie M. |
description | Much research has shown that humans can allocate attentional control differentially to multiple locations based on the amount of conflict historically associated with a given location. Additionally, once established, these control settings can transfer to nearby locations that themselves have no conflict bias. Here we examined if these control settings also extend to nearby locations that are presented outside of the original frame of reference of biased stimuli. During training, participants first responded to biased flanker stimuli that were likely high conflict in one location and low conflict in another location. Then they were exposed to two sets of unbiased stimuli presented in novel transfer locations outside of the established reference frame of biased stimuli. Across three experiments, attentional control settings transferred beyond the reference frame including when there was a visual border (Experiment 2) or meaningful categorical distinction (Experiment 3) delineating training and transfer locations. These novel findings further support the idea that stimulus-driven attention control can be flexibly allocated, perhaps in a categorical manner. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00426-018-0984-9 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_webof</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_webofscience_primary_000511506700019CitationCount</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1990489796</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c372t-8cbb13fe8c6b18d0d245d01aa9d82605e82a375b179802768a8ed77143382b593</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkE1r3DAQhkVpSLbb_IBegqGXQHE6kmx9HMOSfkAgl-QsZHucePFKiSQT9t9HjrehBAo9aRDPO8z7EPKFwgUFkN8jQMVECVSVoFVV6g9kRSsOJZOSfSQr4BWUIJk8IZ9i3AJQKYQ8JidM81oozVdEXaaELg3e2bFovUvBj0UK1sUeQywa3HvXFekBi4D5B12LRR_sDj-To96OEU8P75rc_bi63fwqr29-_t5cXpctlyyVqm0ayntUrWio6qBjVd0BtVZ3igmoUTHLZd1QqRUwKZRV2EmZW3DFmlrzNTlf9j4G_zRhTGY3xBbH0Tr0UzRUa6iUllpk9Os7dOunkIu9UpxTELzKFF2oNvgYcyvzGIadDXtDwcxazaLVZK1m1mrmI84Om6dmh91b4o_HDKgFeMbG97EdZlFvGADUlNYgZJ6o3gzJzsY3fnIpR7_9fzTTbKFjJtw9hr86_vP8F7tUoSs</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1993310634</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Attentional control transfers beyond the reference frame</title><source>SpringerNature Journals</source><source>EBSCOhost Business Source Complete</source><source>Web of Science - Social Sciences Citation Index – 2020<img src="https://exlibris-pub.s3.amazonaws.com/fromwos-v2.jpg" /></source><creator>Weidler, Blaire J. ; Dey, Abhishek ; Bugg, Julie M.</creator><creatorcontrib>Weidler, Blaire J. ; Dey, Abhishek ; Bugg, Julie M.</creatorcontrib><description>Much research has shown that humans can allocate attentional control differentially to multiple locations based on the amount of conflict historically associated with a given location. Additionally, once established, these control settings can transfer to nearby locations that themselves have no conflict bias. Here we examined if these control settings also extend to nearby locations that are presented outside of the original frame of reference of biased stimuli. During training, participants first responded to biased flanker stimuli that were likely high conflict in one location and low conflict in another location. Then they were exposed to two sets of unbiased stimuli presented in novel transfer locations outside of the established reference frame of biased stimuli. Across three experiments, attentional control settings transferred beyond the reference frame including when there was a visual border (Experiment 2) or meaningful categorical distinction (Experiment 3) delineating training and transfer locations. These novel findings further support the idea that stimulus-driven attention control can be flexibly allocated, perhaps in a categorical manner.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0340-0727</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1430-2772</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-0984-9</identifier><identifier>PMID: 29356893</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Attention ; Behavioral Science and Psychology ; Original Article ; Psychology ; Psychology Research ; Psychology, Experimental ; Social Sciences ; Visual task performance</subject><ispartof>Psychological research, 2020-02, Vol.84 (1), p.217-230</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018</rights><rights>Psychological Research is a copyright of Springer, (2018). All Rights Reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>true</woscitedreferencessubscribed><woscitedreferencescount>8</woscitedreferencescount><woscitedreferencesoriginalsourcerecordid>wos000511506700019</woscitedreferencesoriginalsourcerecordid><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c372t-8cbb13fe8c6b18d0d245d01aa9d82605e82a375b179802768a8ed77143382b593</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c372t-8cbb13fe8c6b18d0d245d01aa9d82605e82a375b179802768a8ed77143382b593</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-8825-6091</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00426-018-0984-9$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00426-018-0984-9$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>315,781,785,27929,27930,28254,41493,42562,51324</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29356893$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Weidler, Blaire J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dey, Abhishek</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bugg, Julie M.</creatorcontrib><title>Attentional control transfers beyond the reference frame</title><title>Psychological research</title><addtitle>Psychological Research</addtitle><addtitle>PSYCHOL RES-PSYCH FO</addtitle><addtitle>Psychol Res</addtitle><description>Much research has shown that humans can allocate attentional control differentially to multiple locations based on the amount of conflict historically associated with a given location. Additionally, once established, these control settings can transfer to nearby locations that themselves have no conflict bias. Here we examined if these control settings also extend to nearby locations that are presented outside of the original frame of reference of biased stimuli. During training, participants first responded to biased flanker stimuli that were likely high conflict in one location and low conflict in another location. Then they were exposed to two sets of unbiased stimuli presented in novel transfer locations outside of the established reference frame of biased stimuli. Across three experiments, attentional control settings transferred beyond the reference frame including when there was a visual border (Experiment 2) or meaningful categorical distinction (Experiment 3) delineating training and transfer locations. These novel findings further support the idea that stimulus-driven attention control can be flexibly allocated, perhaps in a categorical manner.</description><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology</subject><subject>Original Article</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology Research</subject><subject>Psychology, Experimental</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Visual task performance</subject><issn>0340-0727</issn><issn>1430-2772</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>ARHDP</sourceid><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkE1r3DAQhkVpSLbb_IBegqGXQHE6kmx9HMOSfkAgl-QsZHucePFKiSQT9t9HjrehBAo9aRDPO8z7EPKFwgUFkN8jQMVECVSVoFVV6g9kRSsOJZOSfSQr4BWUIJk8IZ9i3AJQKYQ8JidM81oozVdEXaaELg3e2bFovUvBj0UK1sUeQywa3HvXFekBi4D5B12LRR_sDj-To96OEU8P75rc_bi63fwqr29-_t5cXpctlyyVqm0ayntUrWio6qBjVd0BtVZ3igmoUTHLZd1QqRUwKZRV2EmZW3DFmlrzNTlf9j4G_zRhTGY3xBbH0Tr0UzRUa6iUllpk9Os7dOunkIu9UpxTELzKFF2oNvgYcyvzGIadDXtDwcxazaLVZK1m1mrmI84Om6dmh91b4o_HDKgFeMbG97EdZlFvGADUlNYgZJ6o3gzJzsY3fnIpR7_9fzTTbKFjJtw9hr86_vP8F7tUoSs</recordid><startdate>20200201</startdate><enddate>20200201</enddate><creator>Weidler, Blaire J.</creator><creator>Dey, Abhishek</creator><creator>Bugg, Julie M.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer Nature</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>17B</scope><scope>ARHDP</scope><scope>BLEPL</scope><scope>DVR</scope><scope>EGQ</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7WY</scope><scope>7WZ</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>87Z</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8FL</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BEZIV</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FRNLG</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>F~G</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K60</scope><scope>K6~</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>L.-</scope><scope>M0C</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PQBIZ</scope><scope>PQBZA</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8825-6091</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20200201</creationdate><title>Attentional control transfers beyond the reference frame</title><author>Weidler, Blaire J. ; Dey, Abhishek ; Bugg, Julie M.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c372t-8cbb13fe8c6b18d0d245d01aa9d82605e82a375b179802768a8ed77143382b593</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Behavioral Science and Psychology</topic><topic>Original Article</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology Research</topic><topic>Psychology, Experimental</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Visual task performance</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Weidler, Blaire J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Dey, Abhishek</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bugg, Julie M.</creatorcontrib><collection>Web of Knowledge</collection><collection>Web of Science - Social Sciences Citation Index – 2020</collection><collection>Web of Science Core Collection</collection><collection>Social Sciences Citation Index</collection><collection>Web of Science Primary (SCIE, SSCI & AHCI)</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Access via ABI/INFORM (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global (PDF only)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Business Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Business Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global (Corporate)</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Business Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Business Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Professional Advanced</collection><collection>ABI/INFORM Global</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Business</collection><collection>ProQuest One Business (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Psychological research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Weidler, Blaire J.</au><au>Dey, Abhishek</au><au>Bugg, Julie M.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Attentional control transfers beyond the reference frame</atitle><jtitle>Psychological research</jtitle><stitle>Psychological Research</stitle><stitle>PSYCHOL RES-PSYCH FO</stitle><addtitle>Psychol Res</addtitle><date>2020-02-01</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>84</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>217</spage><epage>230</epage><pages>217-230</pages><issn>0340-0727</issn><eissn>1430-2772</eissn><abstract>Much research has shown that humans can allocate attentional control differentially to multiple locations based on the amount of conflict historically associated with a given location. Additionally, once established, these control settings can transfer to nearby locations that themselves have no conflict bias. Here we examined if these control settings also extend to nearby locations that are presented outside of the original frame of reference of biased stimuli. During training, participants first responded to biased flanker stimuli that were likely high conflict in one location and low conflict in another location. Then they were exposed to two sets of unbiased stimuli presented in novel transfer locations outside of the established reference frame of biased stimuli. Across three experiments, attentional control settings transferred beyond the reference frame including when there was a visual border (Experiment 2) or meaningful categorical distinction (Experiment 3) delineating training and transfer locations. These novel findings further support the idea that stimulus-driven attention control can be flexibly allocated, perhaps in a categorical manner.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>29356893</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00426-018-0984-9</doi><tpages>14</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8825-6091</orcidid></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0340-0727 |
ispartof | Psychological research, 2020-02, Vol.84 (1), p.217-230 |
issn | 0340-0727 1430-2772 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_webofscience_primary_000511506700019CitationCount |
source | SpringerNature Journals; EBSCOhost Business Source Complete; Web of Science - Social Sciences Citation Index – 2020<img src="https://exlibris-pub.s3.amazonaws.com/fromwos-v2.jpg" /> |
subjects | Attention Behavioral Science and Psychology Original Article Psychology Psychology Research Psychology, Experimental Social Sciences Visual task performance |
title | Attentional control transfers beyond the reference frame |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-11T12%3A07%3A18IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_webof&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Attentional%20control%20transfers%20beyond%20the%20reference%20frame&rft.jtitle=Psychological%20research&rft.au=Weidler,%20Blaire%20J.&rft.date=2020-02-01&rft.volume=84&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=217&rft.epage=230&rft.pages=217-230&rft.issn=0340-0727&rft.eissn=1430-2772&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00426-018-0984-9&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_webof%3E1990489796%3C/proquest_webof%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1993310634&rft_id=info:pmid/29356893&rfr_iscdi=true |