Self-motion perception without sensory motion

Various studies have demonstrated a role for cognition on self-motion perception. Those studies all concerned modulations of the perception of a physical or visual motion stimulus. In our study, however, we investigated whether cognitive cues could elicit a percept of oscillatory self-motion in the...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Experimental brain research 2022-10, Vol.240 (10), p.2677-2685
Hauptverfasser: Reuten, A. J. C., Smeets, J. B. J., Martens, M. H., Bos, J. E.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 2685
container_issue 10
container_start_page 2677
container_title Experimental brain research
container_volume 240
creator Reuten, A. J. C.
Smeets, J. B. J.
Martens, M. H.
Bos, J. E.
description Various studies have demonstrated a role for cognition on self-motion perception. Those studies all concerned modulations of the perception of a physical or visual motion stimulus. In our study, however, we investigated whether cognitive cues could elicit a percept of oscillatory self-motion in the absence of sensory motion. If so, we could use this percept to investigate if the resulting mismatch between estimated self-motion and a lack of corresponding sensory signals is motion sickening. To that end, we seated blindfolded participants on a swing that remained motionless during two conditions, apart from a deliberate perturbation at the start of each condition. The conditions only differed regarding instructions, a secondary task and a demonstration, which suggested either a quick halt (“Distraction”) or continuing oscillations of the swing (“Focus”). Participants reported that the swing oscillated with larger peak-to-peak displacements and for a longer period of time in the Focus condition. That increase was not reflected in the reported motion sickness scores, which did not differ between the two conditions. As the reported motion was rather small, the lack of an effect on the motion sickness response can be explained by assuming a subthreshold neural conflict. Our results support the existence of internal models relevant to sensorimotor processing and the potential of cognitive (behavioral) therapies to alleviate undesirable perceptual issues to some extent. We conclude that oscillatory self-motion can be perceived in the absence of related sensory stimulation, which advocates for the acknowledgement of cognitive cues in studies on self-motion perception.
doi_str_mv 10.1007/s00221-022-06442-3
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>gale_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9510117</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A719645581</galeid><sourcerecordid>A719645581</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c583t-ddb312a91621aecdbfaf407e29b0f0f0e5191c5446e227ec772e4f1c89e30dd13</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kl1rFDEUhoModm39A14tCFIvUnPyMZm5EUqpWigI1l6HbObMbsrMZE1mtP33zXSKdUTkwMnX8775OoS8AXYCjOkPiTHOgeZEWSElp-IZWYEUnAKw4jlZMQaSyhKqA_IqpZtpKDR7SQ6EqspCF3pF6BW2De3C4EO_3mN0uH_o_vLDLozDOmGfQrxbz8QRedHYNuHrx_aQXH86_372hV5-_XxxdnpJnSrFQOt6I4DbCgoOFl29aWwjmUZebViTAxVU4JSUBXKu0WnNUTbgygoFq2sQh-Tj7LsfNx3WDvsh2tbso-9svDPBerNc6f3ObMNPUylgADobHD8axPBjxDSYzieHbWt7DGMyXDNZFkWlJvTtX-hNGGOfr5cp0EpwxconamtbNL5vQt7XTabmVENVSKXK6dwn_6By1Nh5F3psfJ5fCN4vBJkZ8HbY2jElc3H1bcm--4PdoW2HXQrtOH1MWoJ8Bl0MKUVsfj8cMDNVjpkrx-RkHirHiCwSsyhluN9ifHqG_6juAbINwXU</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2717532508</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Self-motion perception without sensory motion</title><source>SpringerLink Journals</source><creator>Reuten, A. J. C. ; Smeets, J. B. J. ; Martens, M. H. ; Bos, J. E.</creator><creatorcontrib>Reuten, A. J. C. ; Smeets, J. B. J. ; Martens, M. H. ; Bos, J. E.</creatorcontrib><description>Various studies have demonstrated a role for cognition on self-motion perception. Those studies all concerned modulations of the perception of a physical or visual motion stimulus. In our study, however, we investigated whether cognitive cues could elicit a percept of oscillatory self-motion in the absence of sensory motion. If so, we could use this percept to investigate if the resulting mismatch between estimated self-motion and a lack of corresponding sensory signals is motion sickening. To that end, we seated blindfolded participants on a swing that remained motionless during two conditions, apart from a deliberate perturbation at the start of each condition. The conditions only differed regarding instructions, a secondary task and a demonstration, which suggested either a quick halt (“Distraction”) or continuing oscillations of the swing (“Focus”). Participants reported that the swing oscillated with larger peak-to-peak displacements and for a longer period of time in the Focus condition. That increase was not reflected in the reported motion sickness scores, which did not differ between the two conditions. As the reported motion was rather small, the lack of an effect on the motion sickness response can be explained by assuming a subthreshold neural conflict. Our results support the existence of internal models relevant to sensorimotor processing and the potential of cognitive (behavioral) therapies to alleviate undesirable perceptual issues to some extent. We conclude that oscillatory self-motion can be perceived in the absence of related sensory stimulation, which advocates for the acknowledgement of cognitive cues in studies on self-motion perception.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-022-06442-3</identifier><identifier>PMID: 35986767</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Analysis ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain stimulation ; Cognitive ability ; Methods ; Motion detection ; Motion perception (Vision) ; Motion sickness ; Neurology ; Neurosciences ; Oscillations ; Perception ; Research Article ; Sensorimotor integration ; Sensorimotor system ; Visual stimuli</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2022-10, Vol.240 (10), p.2677-2685</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2022</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2022 Springer</rights><rights>The Author(s) 2022. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c583t-ddb312a91621aecdbfaf407e29b0f0f0e5191c5446e227ec772e4f1c89e30dd13</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c583t-ddb312a91621aecdbfaf407e29b0f0f0e5191c5446e227ec772e4f1c89e30dd13</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-3794-0579 ; 0000-0002-1661-7019 ; 0000-0003-4641-4180 ; 0000-0002-1494-6804</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00221-022-06442-3$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00221-022-06442-3$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925,41488,42557,51319</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Reuten, A. J. C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smeets, J. B. J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Martens, M. H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bos, J. E.</creatorcontrib><title>Self-motion perception without sensory motion</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>Various studies have demonstrated a role for cognition on self-motion perception. Those studies all concerned modulations of the perception of a physical or visual motion stimulus. In our study, however, we investigated whether cognitive cues could elicit a percept of oscillatory self-motion in the absence of sensory motion. If so, we could use this percept to investigate if the resulting mismatch between estimated self-motion and a lack of corresponding sensory signals is motion sickening. To that end, we seated blindfolded participants on a swing that remained motionless during two conditions, apart from a deliberate perturbation at the start of each condition. The conditions only differed regarding instructions, a secondary task and a demonstration, which suggested either a quick halt (“Distraction”) or continuing oscillations of the swing (“Focus”). Participants reported that the swing oscillated with larger peak-to-peak displacements and for a longer period of time in the Focus condition. That increase was not reflected in the reported motion sickness scores, which did not differ between the two conditions. As the reported motion was rather small, the lack of an effect on the motion sickness response can be explained by assuming a subthreshold neural conflict. Our results support the existence of internal models relevant to sensorimotor processing and the potential of cognitive (behavioral) therapies to alleviate undesirable perceptual issues to some extent. We conclude that oscillatory self-motion can be perceived in the absence of related sensory stimulation, which advocates for the acknowledgement of cognitive cues in studies on self-motion perception.</description><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain stimulation</subject><subject>Cognitive ability</subject><subject>Methods</subject><subject>Motion detection</subject><subject>Motion perception (Vision)</subject><subject>Motion sickness</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Oscillations</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Research Article</subject><subject>Sensorimotor integration</subject><subject>Sensorimotor system</subject><subject>Visual stimuli</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>C6C</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kl1rFDEUhoModm39A14tCFIvUnPyMZm5EUqpWigI1l6HbObMbsrMZE1mtP33zXSKdUTkwMnX8775OoS8AXYCjOkPiTHOgeZEWSElp-IZWYEUnAKw4jlZMQaSyhKqA_IqpZtpKDR7SQ6EqspCF3pF6BW2De3C4EO_3mN0uH_o_vLDLozDOmGfQrxbz8QRedHYNuHrx_aQXH86_372hV5-_XxxdnpJnSrFQOt6I4DbCgoOFl29aWwjmUZebViTAxVU4JSUBXKu0WnNUTbgygoFq2sQh-Tj7LsfNx3WDvsh2tbso-9svDPBerNc6f3ObMNPUylgADobHD8axPBjxDSYzieHbWt7DGMyXDNZFkWlJvTtX-hNGGOfr5cp0EpwxconamtbNL5vQt7XTabmVENVSKXK6dwn_6By1Nh5F3psfJ5fCN4vBJkZ8HbY2jElc3H1bcm--4PdoW2HXQrtOH1MWoJ8Bl0MKUVsfj8cMDNVjpkrx-RkHirHiCwSsyhluN9ifHqG_6juAbINwXU</recordid><startdate>20221001</startdate><enddate>20221001</enddate><creator>Reuten, A. J. C.</creator><creator>Smeets, J. B. J.</creator><creator>Martens, M. H.</creator><creator>Bos, J. E.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3794-0579</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1661-7019</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4641-4180</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1494-6804</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20221001</creationdate><title>Self-motion perception without sensory motion</title><author>Reuten, A. J. C. ; Smeets, J. B. J. ; Martens, M. H. ; Bos, J. E.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c583t-ddb312a91621aecdbfaf407e29b0f0f0e5191c5446e227ec772e4f1c89e30dd13</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain stimulation</topic><topic>Cognitive ability</topic><topic>Methods</topic><topic>Motion detection</topic><topic>Motion perception (Vision)</topic><topic>Motion sickness</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Oscillations</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Research Article</topic><topic>Sensorimotor integration</topic><topic>Sensorimotor system</topic><topic>Visual stimuli</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Reuten, A. J. C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smeets, J. B. J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Martens, M. H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bos, J. E.</creatorcontrib><collection>Springer Nature OA Free Journals</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium &amp; Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Reuten, A. J. C.</au><au>Smeets, J. B. J.</au><au>Martens, M. H.</au><au>Bos, J. E.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Self-motion perception without sensory motion</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><stitle>Exp Brain Res</stitle><date>2022-10-01</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>240</volume><issue>10</issue><spage>2677</spage><epage>2685</epage><pages>2677-2685</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><abstract>Various studies have demonstrated a role for cognition on self-motion perception. Those studies all concerned modulations of the perception of a physical or visual motion stimulus. In our study, however, we investigated whether cognitive cues could elicit a percept of oscillatory self-motion in the absence of sensory motion. If so, we could use this percept to investigate if the resulting mismatch between estimated self-motion and a lack of corresponding sensory signals is motion sickening. To that end, we seated blindfolded participants on a swing that remained motionless during two conditions, apart from a deliberate perturbation at the start of each condition. The conditions only differed regarding instructions, a secondary task and a demonstration, which suggested either a quick halt (“Distraction”) or continuing oscillations of the swing (“Focus”). Participants reported that the swing oscillated with larger peak-to-peak displacements and for a longer period of time in the Focus condition. That increase was not reflected in the reported motion sickness scores, which did not differ between the two conditions. As the reported motion was rather small, the lack of an effect on the motion sickness response can be explained by assuming a subthreshold neural conflict. Our results support the existence of internal models relevant to sensorimotor processing and the potential of cognitive (behavioral) therapies to alleviate undesirable perceptual issues to some extent. We conclude that oscillatory self-motion can be perceived in the absence of related sensory stimulation, which advocates for the acknowledgement of cognitive cues in studies on self-motion perception.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>35986767</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-022-06442-3</doi><tpages>9</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3794-0579</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1661-7019</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4641-4180</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1494-6804</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0014-4819
ispartof Experimental brain research, 2022-10, Vol.240 (10), p.2677-2685
issn 0014-4819
1432-1106
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_9510117
source SpringerLink Journals
subjects Analysis
Biomedical and Life Sciences
Biomedicine
Brain stimulation
Cognitive ability
Methods
Motion detection
Motion perception (Vision)
Motion sickness
Neurology
Neurosciences
Oscillations
Perception
Research Article
Sensorimotor integration
Sensorimotor system
Visual stimuli
title Self-motion perception without sensory motion
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-04T14%3A03%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Self-motion%20perception%20without%20sensory%20motion&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=Reuten,%20A.%20J.%20C.&rft.date=2022-10-01&rft.volume=240&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=2677&rft.epage=2685&rft.pages=2677-2685&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-022-06442-3&rft_dat=%3Cgale_pubme%3EA719645581%3C/gale_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2717532508&rft_id=info:pmid/35986767&rft_galeid=A719645581&rfr_iscdi=true