Fine‐tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants’ detection of language switching
The ability to differentiate between two languages sets the stage for bilingual learning. Infants can discriminate languages when hearing long passages, but language switches often occur on short time scales with few cues to language identity. As bilingual infants begin learning sequences of sounds...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Infancy 2021-11, Vol.26 (6), p.1037-1056 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 1056 |
---|---|
container_issue | 6 |
container_start_page | 1037 |
container_title | Infancy |
container_volume | 26 |
creator | Schott, Esther Mastroberardino, Meghan Fourakis, Eva Lew‐Williams, Casey Byers‐Heinlein, Krista |
description | The ability to differentiate between two languages sets the stage for bilingual learning. Infants can discriminate languages when hearing long passages, but language switches often occur on short time scales with few cues to language identity. As bilingual infants begin learning sequences of sounds and words, how do they detect the dynamics of two languages? In two studies using the head‐turn preference procedure, we investigated whether infants (n = 44) can discriminate languages at the level of individual words. In Study 1, bilingual and monolingual 8‐ to 12‐month‐olds were tested on their detection of single‐word language switching in lists of words (e.g., “dog… lait [fr. milk]”). In Study 2, they were tested on language switching within sentences (e.g., “Do you like the lait?”). We found that infants were unable to detect language switching in lists of words, but the results were inconclusive about infants’ ability to detect language switching within sentences. No differences were observed between bilinguals and monolinguals. Given that bilingual proficiency eventually requires detection of sound sequences across two languages, more research will be needed to conclusively understand when and how this skill emerges. Materials, data, and analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/9dtwn/. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1111/infa.12429 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_8530864</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2569618650</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4489-16a57789b8f6409073cea57f7a37a3382cfd426328aa89f9d4fab6dd1fb681f13</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kctOAyEUhonR2HrZ-ABmEjfGZBQYhgEXJrWx2sToRteEzkClTqEOMxp3fQS3vp5PImOtt4WEhAPny8_5zwFgB8FDFNaRsVoeIkwwXwFdlCY4zmDGVtsYpzGEkHXAhveTEKUEo3XQSQhhmGLSBfcDY9Xb_KVurLHjqJR23Mixigrj88pMjZW1cfY4OjWlaVNlJG0RTZ11y3v7u6392_w1KlSt8paPnP6W8k-mzu8CvQXWtCy92v48N8Ht4OymfxFfXp8P-73LOA9l8RhRmWYZ4yOmKYEcZkmuwovOZBJ2wnCuC4JpgpmUjGteEC1HtCiQHlGGNEo2wclCd9aMpqrIla0rWYpZ8COrZ-GkEb8z1tyJsXsULE0goyQI7H8KVO6hUb4W09AOVQZLyjVe4JRyihhNYUD3_qAT11Q22AsUwxxjmvJAHSyovHLeV0p_FYOgaGco2i6KjxkGePdn-V_ocmgBQAvgyZTq-R8pMbwa9Bai7481qyU</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2582922659</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Fine‐tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants’ detection of language switching</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via Wiley Online Library</source><creator>Schott, Esther ; Mastroberardino, Meghan ; Fourakis, Eva ; Lew‐Williams, Casey ; Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</creator><creatorcontrib>Schott, Esther ; Mastroberardino, Meghan ; Fourakis, Eva ; Lew‐Williams, Casey ; Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</creatorcontrib><description>The ability to differentiate between two languages sets the stage for bilingual learning. Infants can discriminate languages when hearing long passages, but language switches often occur on short time scales with few cues to language identity. As bilingual infants begin learning sequences of sounds and words, how do they detect the dynamics of two languages? In two studies using the head‐turn preference procedure, we investigated whether infants (n = 44) can discriminate languages at the level of individual words. In Study 1, bilingual and monolingual 8‐ to 12‐month‐olds were tested on their detection of single‐word language switching in lists of words (e.g., “dog… lait [fr. milk]”). In Study 2, they were tested on language switching within sentences (e.g., “Do you like the lait?”). We found that infants were unable to detect language switching in lists of words, but the results were inconclusive about infants’ ability to detect language switching within sentences. No differences were observed between bilinguals and monolinguals. Given that bilingual proficiency eventually requires detection of sound sequences across two languages, more research will be needed to conclusively understand when and how this skill emerges. Materials, data, and analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/9dtwn/.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1525-0008</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1532-7078</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/infa.12429</identifier><identifier>PMID: 34482624</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</publisher><subject>Bilingualism ; Humans ; Infant ; infants ; Language ; Language Development ; language discrimination ; Language Proficiency ; language switching ; Learning ; Monolingualism ; Multilingualism ; Speech Perception ; word learning ; Young Children</subject><ispartof>Infancy, 2021-11, Vol.26 (6), p.1037-1056</ispartof><rights>2021 International Congress of Infant Studies.</rights><rights>2021 The International Congress of Infant Studies</rights><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4489-16a57789b8f6409073cea57f7a37a3382cfd426328aa89f9d4fab6dd1fb681f13</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c4489-16a57789b8f6409073cea57f7a37a3382cfd426328aa89f9d4fab6dd1fb681f13</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-0114-4773 ; 0000-0001-8818-1720 ; 0000-0002-8781-4458 ; 0000-0002-7040-2510 ; 0000-0003-3891-7538</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111%2Finfa.12429$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111%2Finfa.12429$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,1417,27924,27925,45574,45575</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34482624$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Schott, Esther</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mastroberardino, Meghan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fourakis, Eva</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lew‐Williams, Casey</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</creatorcontrib><title>Fine‐tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants’ detection of language switching</title><title>Infancy</title><addtitle>Infancy</addtitle><description>The ability to differentiate between two languages sets the stage for bilingual learning. Infants can discriminate languages when hearing long passages, but language switches often occur on short time scales with few cues to language identity. As bilingual infants begin learning sequences of sounds and words, how do they detect the dynamics of two languages? In two studies using the head‐turn preference procedure, we investigated whether infants (n = 44) can discriminate languages at the level of individual words. In Study 1, bilingual and monolingual 8‐ to 12‐month‐olds were tested on their detection of single‐word language switching in lists of words (e.g., “dog… lait [fr. milk]”). In Study 2, they were tested on language switching within sentences (e.g., “Do you like the lait?”). We found that infants were unable to detect language switching in lists of words, but the results were inconclusive about infants’ ability to detect language switching within sentences. No differences were observed between bilinguals and monolinguals. Given that bilingual proficiency eventually requires detection of sound sequences across two languages, more research will be needed to conclusively understand when and how this skill emerges. Materials, data, and analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/9dtwn/.</description><subject>Bilingualism</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infant</subject><subject>infants</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>language discrimination</subject><subject>Language Proficiency</subject><subject>language switching</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Monolingualism</subject><subject>Multilingualism</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><subject>word learning</subject><subject>Young Children</subject><issn>1525-0008</issn><issn>1532-7078</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2021</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kctOAyEUhonR2HrZ-ABmEjfGZBQYhgEXJrWx2sToRteEzkClTqEOMxp3fQS3vp5PImOtt4WEhAPny8_5zwFgB8FDFNaRsVoeIkwwXwFdlCY4zmDGVtsYpzGEkHXAhveTEKUEo3XQSQhhmGLSBfcDY9Xb_KVurLHjqJR23Mixigrj88pMjZW1cfY4OjWlaVNlJG0RTZ11y3v7u6392_w1KlSt8paPnP6W8k-mzu8CvQXWtCy92v48N8Ht4OymfxFfXp8P-73LOA9l8RhRmWYZ4yOmKYEcZkmuwovOZBJ2wnCuC4JpgpmUjGteEC1HtCiQHlGGNEo2wclCd9aMpqrIla0rWYpZ8COrZ-GkEb8z1tyJsXsULE0goyQI7H8KVO6hUb4W09AOVQZLyjVe4JRyihhNYUD3_qAT11Q22AsUwxxjmvJAHSyovHLeV0p_FYOgaGco2i6KjxkGePdn-V_ocmgBQAvgyZTq-R8pMbwa9Bai7481qyU</recordid><startdate>202111</startdate><enddate>202111</enddate><creator>Schott, Esther</creator><creator>Mastroberardino, Meghan</creator><creator>Fourakis, Eva</creator><creator>Lew‐Williams, Casey</creator><creator>Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</creator><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0114-4773</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8818-1720</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8781-4458</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7040-2510</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3891-7538</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202111</creationdate><title>Fine‐tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants’ detection of language switching</title><author>Schott, Esther ; Mastroberardino, Meghan ; Fourakis, Eva ; Lew‐Williams, Casey ; Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c4489-16a57789b8f6409073cea57f7a37a3382cfd426328aa89f9d4fab6dd1fb681f13</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2021</creationdate><topic>Bilingualism</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infant</topic><topic>infants</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>language discrimination</topic><topic>Language Proficiency</topic><topic>language switching</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Monolingualism</topic><topic>Multilingualism</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><topic>word learning</topic><topic>Young Children</topic><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Schott, Esther</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mastroberardino, Meghan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fourakis, Eva</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lew‐Williams, Casey</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Infancy</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Schott, Esther</au><au>Mastroberardino, Meghan</au><au>Fourakis, Eva</au><au>Lew‐Williams, Casey</au><au>Byers‐Heinlein, Krista</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Fine‐tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants’ detection of language switching</atitle><jtitle>Infancy</jtitle><addtitle>Infancy</addtitle><date>2021-11</date><risdate>2021</risdate><volume>26</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>1037</spage><epage>1056</epage><pages>1037-1056</pages><issn>1525-0008</issn><eissn>1532-7078</eissn><abstract>The ability to differentiate between two languages sets the stage for bilingual learning. Infants can discriminate languages when hearing long passages, but language switches often occur on short time scales with few cues to language identity. As bilingual infants begin learning sequences of sounds and words, how do they detect the dynamics of two languages? In two studies using the head‐turn preference procedure, we investigated whether infants (n = 44) can discriminate languages at the level of individual words. In Study 1, bilingual and monolingual 8‐ to 12‐month‐olds were tested on their detection of single‐word language switching in lists of words (e.g., “dog… lait [fr. milk]”). In Study 2, they were tested on language switching within sentences (e.g., “Do you like the lait?”). We found that infants were unable to detect language switching in lists of words, but the results were inconclusive about infants’ ability to detect language switching within sentences. No differences were observed between bilinguals and monolinguals. Given that bilingual proficiency eventually requires detection of sound sequences across two languages, more research will be needed to conclusively understand when and how this skill emerges. Materials, data, and analysis scripts are available at https://osf.io/9dtwn/.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</pub><pmid>34482624</pmid><doi>10.1111/infa.12429</doi><tpages>20</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0114-4773</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8818-1720</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8781-4458</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7040-2510</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3891-7538</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1525-0008 |
ispartof | Infancy, 2021-11, Vol.26 (6), p.1037-1056 |
issn | 1525-0008 1532-7078 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_8530864 |
source | MEDLINE; Access via Wiley Online Library |
subjects | Bilingualism Humans Infant infants Language Language Development language discrimination Language Proficiency language switching Learning Monolingualism Multilingualism Speech Perception word learning Young Children |
title | Fine‐tuning language discrimination: Bilingual and monolingual infants’ detection of language switching |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-22T08%3A56%3A21IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Fine%E2%80%90tuning%20language%20discrimination:%20Bilingual%20and%20monolingual%20infants%E2%80%99%20detection%20of%20language%20switching&rft.jtitle=Infancy&rft.au=Schott,%20Esther&rft.date=2021-11&rft.volume=26&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=1037&rft.epage=1056&rft.pages=1037-1056&rft.issn=1525-0008&rft.eissn=1532-7078&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/infa.12429&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2569618650%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2582922659&rft_id=info:pmid/34482624&rfr_iscdi=true |