Listeners are better at predicting speakers similar to themselves
Although it takes several hundred milliseconds to prepare a spoken contribution, gaps between turns in conversation tend to be much shorter. To produce these short gaps, it appears that interlocutors predict the end of their partner's turn. The theory of prediction-by-simulation proposes that i...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Acta psychologica 2020-07, Vol.208, p.103094-103094, Article 103094 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 103094 |
---|---|
container_issue | |
container_start_page | 103094 |
container_title | Acta psychologica |
container_volume | 208 |
creator | Hadley, Lauren V. Fisher, Nina K. Pickering, Martin J. |
description | Although it takes several hundred milliseconds to prepare a spoken contribution, gaps between turns in conversation tend to be much shorter. To produce these short gaps, it appears that interlocutors predict the end of their partner's turn. The theory of prediction-by-simulation proposes that individuals use their own motor system to model a partner's upcoming actions by referring to prior production experience. In this study we investigate the role of motor experience for both predicting a turn-end and producing a spoken response by manipulating the similarity of heard speech to participants' own production style. We hypothesised that they would be better at predicting, and initiating responses to, speech produced in the style they speak themselves. Participants recorded a series of questions in two sessions, and several months later they listened to their own speech and that of a stylistically similar and a stylistically dissimilar participant (as assessed by independent raters). Participants predicted the end of 60 of these questions by pressing a button, and for the remaining 60 questions, by producing a spoken response. An analysis of response times showed that participants' button-press responses were faster for utterances spoken by themselves and by a stylistically similar partner, than for utterances spoken by a stylistically dissimilar partner. We conclude that simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.
•Prediction-by-simulation proposes that prediction is based on one's motor experience.•If this is the case, predictions should be better for people more like oneself.•We tested if speech-style similarity affected prediction during speech listening.•Similarity led to better predictions of turn-ends, but not quicker turn-taking.•We argue simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103094 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_7408002</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0001691820300238</els_id><sourcerecordid>2456878502</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c491t-c4187c16df29038f20f850b695e3b3efccd374f13f4d34979a42bc845104199c3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kUlPBCEQhYnR6Lj8A2M68eJlRgrohYuJmbglk3jRM6HpamXsTWAm8d9LZ9wPXiDAq1ev-Ag5BjoDCtn5cqZNGPzbjFE2XnEqxRaZQJHzacZkvk0mlFKYZhKKPbLv_TIeBUjYJXucpQw4hQm5XFgfsEPnE-0wKTEEdIkOyeCwsibY7inxA-qXUeFtaxvtktAn4Rlbj80a_SHZqXXj8ehjPyCP11cP89vp4v7mbn65mBohIcQ1JjOQVTWTlBc1o3WR0jKTKfKSY21MxXNRA69FxYXMpRasNIVIYQwtDT8gFxvfYVW2WBnsgtONGpxttXtTvbbq90tnn9VTv1a5oAWlLBqcfRi4_nWFPqjWeoNNozvsV14xAYxB7Cyi9PSPdNmvXBfHi6o0K_IYfTQUG5VxvfcO668wQNXISC3VhpEaGakNo1h28nOQr6JPKN-TYvzOtUWnvLHYmQjEoQmq6u3_Hd4BuA2kUQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2456878502</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Listeners are better at predicting speakers similar to themselves</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Hadley, Lauren V. ; Fisher, Nina K. ; Pickering, Martin J.</creator><creatorcontrib>Hadley, Lauren V. ; Fisher, Nina K. ; Pickering, Martin J.</creatorcontrib><description>Although it takes several hundred milliseconds to prepare a spoken contribution, gaps between turns in conversation tend to be much shorter. To produce these short gaps, it appears that interlocutors predict the end of their partner's turn. The theory of prediction-by-simulation proposes that individuals use their own motor system to model a partner's upcoming actions by referring to prior production experience. In this study we investigate the role of motor experience for both predicting a turn-end and producing a spoken response by manipulating the similarity of heard speech to participants' own production style. We hypothesised that they would be better at predicting, and initiating responses to, speech produced in the style they speak themselves. Participants recorded a series of questions in two sessions, and several months later they listened to their own speech and that of a stylistically similar and a stylistically dissimilar participant (as assessed by independent raters). Participants predicted the end of 60 of these questions by pressing a button, and for the remaining 60 questions, by producing a spoken response. An analysis of response times showed that participants' button-press responses were faster for utterances spoken by themselves and by a stylistically similar partner, than for utterances spoken by a stylistically dissimilar partner. We conclude that simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.
•Prediction-by-simulation proposes that prediction is based on one's motor experience.•If this is the case, predictions should be better for people more like oneself.•We tested if speech-style similarity affected prediction during speech listening.•Similarity led to better predictions of turn-ends, but not quicker turn-taking.•We argue simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0001-6918</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-6297</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103094</identifier><identifier>PMID: 32521301</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Netherlands: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Auditory Perception ; Communication ; Conversation ; Hearing ; Humans ; Prediction ; Reaction Time ; Simulation ; Speech ; Speech style ; Turn-taking</subject><ispartof>Acta psychologica, 2020-07, Vol.208, p.103094-103094, Article 103094</ispartof><rights>2020 The Authors</rights><rights>Copyright © 2020 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.</rights><rights>Copyright Elsevier Science Ltd. Jul 2020</rights><rights>2020 The Authors 2020</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c491t-c4187c16df29038f20f850b695e3b3efccd374f13f4d34979a42bc845104199c3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c491t-c4187c16df29038f20f850b695e3b3efccd374f13f4d34979a42bc845104199c3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103094$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,315,781,785,886,3551,27871,27926,27927,45997</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32521301$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hadley, Lauren V.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fisher, Nina K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pickering, Martin J.</creatorcontrib><title>Listeners are better at predicting speakers similar to themselves</title><title>Acta psychologica</title><addtitle>Acta Psychol (Amst)</addtitle><description>Although it takes several hundred milliseconds to prepare a spoken contribution, gaps between turns in conversation tend to be much shorter. To produce these short gaps, it appears that interlocutors predict the end of their partner's turn. The theory of prediction-by-simulation proposes that individuals use their own motor system to model a partner's upcoming actions by referring to prior production experience. In this study we investigate the role of motor experience for both predicting a turn-end and producing a spoken response by manipulating the similarity of heard speech to participants' own production style. We hypothesised that they would be better at predicting, and initiating responses to, speech produced in the style they speak themselves. Participants recorded a series of questions in two sessions, and several months later they listened to their own speech and that of a stylistically similar and a stylistically dissimilar participant (as assessed by independent raters). Participants predicted the end of 60 of these questions by pressing a button, and for the remaining 60 questions, by producing a spoken response. An analysis of response times showed that participants' button-press responses were faster for utterances spoken by themselves and by a stylistically similar partner, than for utterances spoken by a stylistically dissimilar partner. We conclude that simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.
•Prediction-by-simulation proposes that prediction is based on one's motor experience.•If this is the case, predictions should be better for people more like oneself.•We tested if speech-style similarity affected prediction during speech listening.•Similarity led to better predictions of turn-ends, but not quicker turn-taking.•We argue simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.</description><subject>Auditory Perception</subject><subject>Communication</subject><subject>Conversation</subject><subject>Hearing</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Prediction</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Simulation</subject><subject>Speech</subject><subject>Speech style</subject><subject>Turn-taking</subject><issn>0001-6918</issn><issn>1873-6297</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kUlPBCEQhYnR6Lj8A2M68eJlRgrohYuJmbglk3jRM6HpamXsTWAm8d9LZ9wPXiDAq1ev-Ag5BjoDCtn5cqZNGPzbjFE2XnEqxRaZQJHzacZkvk0mlFKYZhKKPbLv_TIeBUjYJXucpQw4hQm5XFgfsEPnE-0wKTEEdIkOyeCwsibY7inxA-qXUeFtaxvtktAn4Rlbj80a_SHZqXXj8ehjPyCP11cP89vp4v7mbn65mBohIcQ1JjOQVTWTlBc1o3WR0jKTKfKSY21MxXNRA69FxYXMpRasNIVIYQwtDT8gFxvfYVW2WBnsgtONGpxttXtTvbbq90tnn9VTv1a5oAWlLBqcfRi4_nWFPqjWeoNNozvsV14xAYxB7Cyi9PSPdNmvXBfHi6o0K_IYfTQUG5VxvfcO668wQNXISC3VhpEaGakNo1h28nOQr6JPKN-TYvzOtUWnvLHYmQjEoQmq6u3_Hd4BuA2kUQ</recordid><startdate>202007</startdate><enddate>202007</enddate><creator>Hadley, Lauren V.</creator><creator>Fisher, Nina K.</creator><creator>Pickering, Martin J.</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier Science Ltd</general><general>North Holland Publishing</general><scope>6I.</scope><scope>AAFTH</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ICWRT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>202007</creationdate><title>Listeners are better at predicting speakers similar to themselves</title><author>Hadley, Lauren V. ; Fisher, Nina K. ; Pickering, Martin J.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c491t-c4187c16df29038f20f850b695e3b3efccd374f13f4d34979a42bc845104199c3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Auditory Perception</topic><topic>Communication</topic><topic>Conversation</topic><topic>Hearing</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Prediction</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Simulation</topic><topic>Speech</topic><topic>Speech style</topic><topic>Turn-taking</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hadley, Lauren V.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fisher, Nina K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pickering, Martin J.</creatorcontrib><collection>ScienceDirect Open Access Titles</collection><collection>Elsevier:ScienceDirect:Open Access</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 28</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Acta psychologica</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hadley, Lauren V.</au><au>Fisher, Nina K.</au><au>Pickering, Martin J.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Listeners are better at predicting speakers similar to themselves</atitle><jtitle>Acta psychologica</jtitle><addtitle>Acta Psychol (Amst)</addtitle><date>2020-07</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>208</volume><spage>103094</spage><epage>103094</epage><pages>103094-103094</pages><artnum>103094</artnum><issn>0001-6918</issn><eissn>1873-6297</eissn><abstract>Although it takes several hundred milliseconds to prepare a spoken contribution, gaps between turns in conversation tend to be much shorter. To produce these short gaps, it appears that interlocutors predict the end of their partner's turn. The theory of prediction-by-simulation proposes that individuals use their own motor system to model a partner's upcoming actions by referring to prior production experience. In this study we investigate the role of motor experience for both predicting a turn-end and producing a spoken response by manipulating the similarity of heard speech to participants' own production style. We hypothesised that they would be better at predicting, and initiating responses to, speech produced in the style they speak themselves. Participants recorded a series of questions in two sessions, and several months later they listened to their own speech and that of a stylistically similar and a stylistically dissimilar participant (as assessed by independent raters). Participants predicted the end of 60 of these questions by pressing a button, and for the remaining 60 questions, by producing a spoken response. An analysis of response times showed that participants' button-press responses were faster for utterances spoken by themselves and by a stylistically similar partner, than for utterances spoken by a stylistically dissimilar partner. We conclude that simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.
•Prediction-by-simulation proposes that prediction is based on one's motor experience.•If this is the case, predictions should be better for people more like oneself.•We tested if speech-style similarity affected prediction during speech listening.•Similarity led to better predictions of turn-ends, but not quicker turn-taking.•We argue simulation facilitates prediction of similar speakers.</abstract><cop>Netherlands</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>32521301</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103094</doi><tpages>1</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0001-6918 |
ispartof | Acta psychologica, 2020-07, Vol.208, p.103094-103094, Article 103094 |
issn | 0001-6918 1873-6297 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_7408002 |
source | MEDLINE; Periodicals Index Online; Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier) |
subjects | Auditory Perception Communication Conversation Hearing Humans Prediction Reaction Time Simulation Speech Speech style Turn-taking |
title | Listeners are better at predicting speakers similar to themselves |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-17T23%3A45%3A38IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Listeners%20are%20better%20at%20predicting%20speakers%20similar%20to%20themselves&rft.jtitle=Acta%20psychologica&rft.au=Hadley,%20Lauren%20V.&rft.date=2020-07&rft.volume=208&rft.spage=103094&rft.epage=103094&rft.pages=103094-103094&rft.artnum=103094&rft.issn=0001-6918&rft.eissn=1873-6297&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103094&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2456878502%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2456878502&rft_id=info:pmid/32521301&rft_els_id=S0001691820300238&rfr_iscdi=true |