Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between
Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Experimental brain research 2017-12, Vol.235 (12), p.3733-3742 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 3742 |
---|---|
container_issue | 12 |
container_start_page | 3733 |
container_title | Experimental brain research |
container_volume | 235 |
creator | Knox, Paul C. Wolohan, Felicity D. A. Helmy, Mai S. |
description | Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when the saccade target appears), there are some healthy, adult participants, “express saccade makers” (ESMs), who persist in generating high proportions (> 30%) of express saccades in overlap conditions. These participants are encountered much more frequently in Chinese participant groups than amongst the Caucasian participants tested to date. What is not known is whether this high number of ESMs is only a feature of Chinese participant groups. More broadly, there are few comparative studies of saccade behaviour across large participant groups drawn from different populations. We, therefore, tested an independent group of 70 healthy adult Egyptian participants, using the same equipment and procedures as employed in the previous studies. Each participant was exposed to two blocks of 200 gap, and two blocks of 200 overlap trials, with block order counterbalanced. Results from the Schwartz Value Survey were used to confirm that this group of participants was culturally distinct from the Chinese and Caucasian (white British) groups tested previously. Fourteen percent (10/70) of this new group were ESMs, and the pattern of latency distribution in these ESMs was identical to that identified in the other participant groups, with a prominent peak in the express latency range in overlap conditions. Overall, we identified three modes in the distribution of saccade latency in overlap conditions, the timing of which (express peak at 110 ms, subsequent peaks at 160 and 210 ms) were strikingly consistent with our previous observations. That these behavioural patterns of saccade latency are observed consistently in large participant groups, drawn from geographically, ethnically, and culturally distinct populations, suggests that they relate to the underlying architecture of the saccade system. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5671528</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A513338737</galeid><sourcerecordid>A513338737</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c602t-581d58057c533724bfa6bc493401e867355f31ed8e85f02c72c59dd7be42de6e3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kt1r1TAYxoMo7mz6B3gjBUEU7Mxn03ghjDl1MBD8uA5p-vacjJ6kNqnb_ntTz5ynorlISPJ7nvC-eRB6QvAxwVi-jhhTSkpMZCmw4iW5h1aEM1oSgqv7aIUx4SWviTpAhzFezlsm8UN0QGslKqzoCr07ux5GiLGIxlrTQiycL1oXk_M2FUMYpt4kF3x8U4CJ6VVxBfNsfJvBsoF0BeAfoQed6SM8vl2P0Lf3Z19PP5YXnz6cn55clLbCNJWiJq2osZBWMCYpbzpTNZYrxjGBupJMiI4RaGuoRYepldQK1bayAU5bqIAdobc732FqttBa8Gk0vR5GtzXjjQ7G6eWNdxu9Dj-0qCQRtM4GL24NxvB9ypXorYsW-t54CFPURPE8sBI4o8_-Qi_DNPpcXqYqzDilSv2h1qYH7XwX8rt2NtUngjDGaslkpo7_QZm531tng4fO5fOF4OVCkJkE12ltphj1-ZfPS_b5HrsB06dNDP3069eWINmBdgwxjtDdNY5gPedJ7_Kkc570nCdNsubpfsfvFL8DlAG6A2K-8msY99r0X9efFKHRZg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1960342299</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerNature Complete Journals</source><creator>Knox, Paul C. ; Wolohan, Felicity D. A. ; Helmy, Mai S.</creator><creatorcontrib>Knox, Paul C. ; Wolohan, Felicity D. A. ; Helmy, Mai S.</creatorcontrib><description>Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when the saccade target appears), there are some healthy, adult participants, “express saccade makers” (ESMs), who persist in generating high proportions (> 30%) of express saccades in overlap conditions. These participants are encountered much more frequently in Chinese participant groups than amongst the Caucasian participants tested to date. What is not known is whether this high number of ESMs is only a feature of Chinese participant groups. More broadly, there are few comparative studies of saccade behaviour across large participant groups drawn from different populations. We, therefore, tested an independent group of 70 healthy adult Egyptian participants, using the same equipment and procedures as employed in the previous studies. Each participant was exposed to two blocks of 200 gap, and two blocks of 200 overlap trials, with block order counterbalanced. Results from the Schwartz Value Survey were used to confirm that this group of participants was culturally distinct from the Chinese and Caucasian (white British) groups tested previously. Fourteen percent (10/70) of this new group were ESMs, and the pattern of latency distribution in these ESMs was identical to that identified in the other participant groups, with a prominent peak in the express latency range in overlap conditions. Overall, we identified three modes in the distribution of saccade latency in overlap conditions, the timing of which (express peak at 110 ms, subsequent peaks at 160 and 210 ms) were strikingly consistent with our previous observations. That these behavioural patterns of saccade latency are observed consistently in large participant groups, drawn from geographically, ethnically, and culturally distinct populations, suggests that they relate to the underlying architecture of the saccade system.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1</identifier><identifier>PMID: 28956092</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; African Continental Ancestry Group ; Analysis of Variance ; Asian Continental Ancestry Group ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain research ; Cultural Diversity ; European Continental Ancestry Group ; Female ; Fixation, Ocular ; Humans ; Latency ; Male ; Neurology ; Neurosciences ; Photic Stimulation ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Research Article ; Saccades (Eye movements) ; Saccades - physiology ; Saccadic eye movements ; Surveys and Questionnaires ; Time Factors ; Values ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2017-12, Vol.235 (12), p.3733-3742</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2017</rights><rights>COPYRIGHT 2017 Springer</rights><rights>Experimental Brain Research is a copyright of Springer, 2017.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c602t-581d58057c533724bfa6bc493401e867355f31ed8e85f02c72c59dd7be42de6e3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c602t-581d58057c533724bfa6bc493401e867355f31ed8e85f02c72c59dd7be42de6e3</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-2578-7335 ; 0000-0002-8403-1730</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,776,780,881,27901,27902,41464,42533,51294</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28956092$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Knox, Paul C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wolohan, Felicity D. A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Helmy, Mai S.</creatorcontrib><title>Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when the saccade target appears), there are some healthy, adult participants, “express saccade makers” (ESMs), who persist in generating high proportions (> 30%) of express saccades in overlap conditions. These participants are encountered much more frequently in Chinese participant groups than amongst the Caucasian participants tested to date. What is not known is whether this high number of ESMs is only a feature of Chinese participant groups. More broadly, there are few comparative studies of saccade behaviour across large participant groups drawn from different populations. We, therefore, tested an independent group of 70 healthy adult Egyptian participants, using the same equipment and procedures as employed in the previous studies. Each participant was exposed to two blocks of 200 gap, and two blocks of 200 overlap trials, with block order counterbalanced. Results from the Schwartz Value Survey were used to confirm that this group of participants was culturally distinct from the Chinese and Caucasian (white British) groups tested previously. Fourteen percent (10/70) of this new group were ESMs, and the pattern of latency distribution in these ESMs was identical to that identified in the other participant groups, with a prominent peak in the express latency range in overlap conditions. Overall, we identified three modes in the distribution of saccade latency in overlap conditions, the timing of which (express peak at 110 ms, subsequent peaks at 160 and 210 ms) were strikingly consistent with our previous observations. That these behavioural patterns of saccade latency are observed consistently in large participant groups, drawn from geographically, ethnically, and culturally distinct populations, suggests that they relate to the underlying architecture of the saccade system.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>African Continental Ancestry Group</subject><subject>Analysis of Variance</subject><subject>Asian Continental Ancestry Group</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain research</subject><subject>Cultural Diversity</subject><subject>European Continental Ancestry Group</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fixation, Ocular</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Latency</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Research Article</subject><subject>Saccades (Eye movements)</subject><subject>Saccades - physiology</subject><subject>Saccadic eye movements</subject><subject>Surveys and Questionnaires</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><subject>Values</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2017</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>C6C</sourceid><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kt1r1TAYxoMo7mz6B3gjBUEU7Mxn03ghjDl1MBD8uA5p-vacjJ6kNqnb_ntTz5ynorlISPJ7nvC-eRB6QvAxwVi-jhhTSkpMZCmw4iW5h1aEM1oSgqv7aIUx4SWviTpAhzFezlsm8UN0QGslKqzoCr07ux5GiLGIxlrTQiycL1oXk_M2FUMYpt4kF3x8U4CJ6VVxBfNsfJvBsoF0BeAfoQed6SM8vl2P0Lf3Z19PP5YXnz6cn55clLbCNJWiJq2osZBWMCYpbzpTNZYrxjGBupJMiI4RaGuoRYepldQK1bayAU5bqIAdobc732FqttBa8Gk0vR5GtzXjjQ7G6eWNdxu9Dj-0qCQRtM4GL24NxvB9ypXorYsW-t54CFPURPE8sBI4o8_-Qi_DNPpcXqYqzDilSv2h1qYH7XwX8rt2NtUngjDGaslkpo7_QZm531tng4fO5fOF4OVCkJkE12ltphj1-ZfPS_b5HrsB06dNDP3069eWINmBdgwxjtDdNY5gPedJ7_Kkc570nCdNsubpfsfvFL8DlAG6A2K-8msY99r0X9efFKHRZg</recordid><startdate>20171201</startdate><enddate>20171201</enddate><creator>Knox, Paul C.</creator><creator>Wolohan, Felicity D. A.</creator><creator>Helmy, Mai S.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ISR</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2578-7335</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8403-1730</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20171201</creationdate><title>Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between</title><author>Knox, Paul C. ; Wolohan, Felicity D. A. ; Helmy, Mai S.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c602t-581d58057c533724bfa6bc493401e867355f31ed8e85f02c72c59dd7be42de6e3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2017</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>African Continental Ancestry Group</topic><topic>Analysis of Variance</topic><topic>Asian Continental Ancestry Group</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain research</topic><topic>Cultural Diversity</topic><topic>European Continental Ancestry Group</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fixation, Ocular</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Latency</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Research Article</topic><topic>Saccades (Eye movements)</topic><topic>Saccades - physiology</topic><topic>Saccadic eye movements</topic><topic>Surveys and Questionnaires</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><topic>Values</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Knox, Paul C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wolohan, Felicity D. A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Helmy, Mai S.</creatorcontrib><collection>Springer Nature OA Free Journals</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Gale In Context: Science</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Proquest Nursing & Allied Health Source</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Knox, Paul C.</au><au>Wolohan, Felicity D. A.</au><au>Helmy, Mai S.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><stitle>Exp Brain Res</stitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2017-12-01</date><risdate>2017</risdate><volume>235</volume><issue>12</issue><spage>3733</spage><epage>3742</epage><pages>3733-3742</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><abstract>Express saccades are low latency (80–130 ms), visually guided saccades. While their occurrence is encouraged by the use of gap tasks (the fixation target is extinguished 200 ms prior to the saccade target appearing) and suppressed by the use of overlap tasks (the fixation target remains present when the saccade target appears), there are some healthy, adult participants, “express saccade makers” (ESMs), who persist in generating high proportions (> 30%) of express saccades in overlap conditions. These participants are encountered much more frequently in Chinese participant groups than amongst the Caucasian participants tested to date. What is not known is whether this high number of ESMs is only a feature of Chinese participant groups. More broadly, there are few comparative studies of saccade behaviour across large participant groups drawn from different populations. We, therefore, tested an independent group of 70 healthy adult Egyptian participants, using the same equipment and procedures as employed in the previous studies. Each participant was exposed to two blocks of 200 gap, and two blocks of 200 overlap trials, with block order counterbalanced. Results from the Schwartz Value Survey were used to confirm that this group of participants was culturally distinct from the Chinese and Caucasian (white British) groups tested previously. Fourteen percent (10/70) of this new group were ESMs, and the pattern of latency distribution in these ESMs was identical to that identified in the other participant groups, with a prominent peak in the express latency range in overlap conditions. Overall, we identified three modes in the distribution of saccade latency in overlap conditions, the timing of which (express peak at 110 ms, subsequent peaks at 160 and 210 ms) were strikingly consistent with our previous observations. That these behavioural patterns of saccade latency are observed consistently in large participant groups, drawn from geographically, ethnically, and culturally distinct populations, suggests that they relate to the underlying architecture of the saccade system.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>28956092</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1</doi><tpages>10</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2578-7335</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8403-1730</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0014-4819 |
ispartof | Experimental brain research, 2017-12, Vol.235 (12), p.3733-3742 |
issn | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5671528 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerNature Complete Journals |
subjects | Adolescent African Continental Ancestry Group Analysis of Variance Asian Continental Ancestry Group Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedicine Brain research Cultural Diversity European Continental Ancestry Group Female Fixation, Ocular Humans Latency Male Neurology Neurosciences Photic Stimulation Reaction Time - physiology Research Article Saccades (Eye movements) Saccades - physiology Saccadic eye movements Surveys and Questionnaires Time Factors Values Young Adult |
title | Express saccades in distinct populations: east, west, and in-between |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-29T02%3A04%3A22IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Express%20saccades%20in%20distinct%20populations:%20east,%20west,%20and%20in-between&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=Knox,%20Paul%20C.&rft.date=2017-12-01&rft.volume=235&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=3733&rft.epage=3742&rft.pages=3733-3742&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-017-5094-1&rft_dat=%3Cgale_pubme%3EA513338737%3C/gale_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1960342299&rft_id=info:pmid/28956092&rft_galeid=A513338737&rfr_iscdi=true |