The Effect of Residual Acoustic Hearing and Adaptation to Uncertainty on Speech Perception in Cochlear Implant Users: Evidence From Eye-Tracking

OBJECTIVES:While outcomes with cochlear implants (CIs) are generally good, performance can be fragile. The authors examined two factors that are crucial for good CI performance. First, while there is a clear benefit for adding residual acoustic hearing to CI stimulation (typically in low frequencies...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Ear and hearing 2016-01, Vol.37 (1), p.e37-e51
Hauptverfasser: McMurray, Bob, Farris-Trimble, Ashley, Seedorff, Michael, Rigler, Hannah
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page e51
container_issue 1
container_start_page e37
container_title Ear and hearing
container_volume 37
creator McMurray, Bob
Farris-Trimble, Ashley
Seedorff, Michael
Rigler, Hannah
description OBJECTIVES:While outcomes with cochlear implants (CIs) are generally good, performance can be fragile. The authors examined two factors that are crucial for good CI performance. First, while there is a clear benefit for adding residual acoustic hearing to CI stimulation (typically in low frequencies), it is unclear whether this contributes directly to phonetic categorization. Thus, the authors examined perception of voicing (which uses low-frequency acoustic cues) and fricative place of articulation (s/∫, which does not) in CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. Second, in speech categorization experiments, CI users typically show shallower identification functions. These are typically interpreted as deriving from noisy encoding of the signal. However, psycholinguistic work suggests shallow slopes may also be a useful way to adapt to uncertainty. The authors thus employed an eye-tracking paradigm to examine this in CI users. DESIGN:Participants were 30 CI users (with a variety of configurations) and 22 age-matched normal hearing (NH) controls. Participants heard tokens from six b/p and six s/∫ continua (eight steps) spanning real words (e.g., beach/peach, sip/ship). Participants selected the picture corresponding to the word they heard from a screen containing four items (a b-, p-, s- and ∫-initial item). Eye movements to each object were monitored as a measure of how strongly they were considering each interpretation in the moments leading up to their final percept. RESULTS:Mouse-click results (analogous to phoneme identification) for voicing showed a shallower slope for CI users than NH listeners, but no differences between CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. For fricatives, CI users also showed a shallower slope, but unexpectedly, acoustic + electric listeners showed an even shallower slope. Eye movements showed a gradient response to fine-grained acoustic differences for all listeners. Even considering only trials in which a participant clicked “b” (for example), and accounting for variation in the category boundary, participants made more looks to the competitor (“p”) as the voice onset time neared the boundary. CI users showed a similar pattern, but looked to the competitor more than NH listeners, and this was not different at different continuum steps. CONCLUSION:Residual acoustic hearing did not improve voicing categorization suggesting it may not help identify these phonetic cues. The fact that acoustic + electric us
doi_str_mv 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000207
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4717908</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1750434414</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3607-5ee4a9525e8d66e46566fa0653d7167b1f64f2401083add81daa06dad05afed03</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kcFu1DAQhi0EosvCGyDkI5cUO3HshAPSarullSqBYPdsufakMXXiYDut9i14ZNxuqQoHfLE08__f2PMj9JaSY0pa8WG1OzkmT09JxDO0oHXVFIxz8RwtCG15kevlEXoV4w9CaNly9hIdlbyiomybBfq17QFvug50wr7D3yBaMyuHV9rPMVmNz0AFO15hNRq8MmpKKlk_4uTxbtQQkrJj2uNc-T4B6B5_haBhutfYEa-97l0m4PNhcmpMeBchxI94c2MNZD8-DX7Amz0U26D0dR70Gr3olIvw5uFeot3pZrs-Ky6-fD5fry4KXXEiihqAqbYua2gM58B4zXmnCK8rIygXl7TjrCsZoaSplDENNSp3jTKkVh0YUi3RpwN3mi8HMBrGFJSTU7CDCnvplZV_d0bbyyt_I5mgos3UJXr_AAj-5wwxycFGDS5_E_LuJBU1YRVjlGUpO0h18DEG6B7HUCLvwpQ5TPlvmNn27ukTH01_0suC5iC49S7lvV67-RaC7EG51P-f_Rt0Dq2f</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1750434414</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The Effect of Residual Acoustic Hearing and Adaptation to Uncertainty on Speech Perception in Cochlear Implant Users: Evidence From Eye-Tracking</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Journals@Ovid Complete</source><creator>McMurray, Bob ; Farris-Trimble, Ashley ; Seedorff, Michael ; Rigler, Hannah</creator><creatorcontrib>McMurray, Bob ; Farris-Trimble, Ashley ; Seedorff, Michael ; Rigler, Hannah</creatorcontrib><description>OBJECTIVES:While outcomes with cochlear implants (CIs) are generally good, performance can be fragile. The authors examined two factors that are crucial for good CI performance. First, while there is a clear benefit for adding residual acoustic hearing to CI stimulation (typically in low frequencies), it is unclear whether this contributes directly to phonetic categorization. Thus, the authors examined perception of voicing (which uses low-frequency acoustic cues) and fricative place of articulation (s/∫, which does not) in CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. Second, in speech categorization experiments, CI users typically show shallower identification functions. These are typically interpreted as deriving from noisy encoding of the signal. However, psycholinguistic work suggests shallow slopes may also be a useful way to adapt to uncertainty. The authors thus employed an eye-tracking paradigm to examine this in CI users. DESIGN:Participants were 30 CI users (with a variety of configurations) and 22 age-matched normal hearing (NH) controls. Participants heard tokens from six b/p and six s/∫ continua (eight steps) spanning real words (e.g., beach/peach, sip/ship). Participants selected the picture corresponding to the word they heard from a screen containing four items (a b-, p-, s- and ∫-initial item). Eye movements to each object were monitored as a measure of how strongly they were considering each interpretation in the moments leading up to their final percept. RESULTS:Mouse-click results (analogous to phoneme identification) for voicing showed a shallower slope for CI users than NH listeners, but no differences between CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. For fricatives, CI users also showed a shallower slope, but unexpectedly, acoustic + electric listeners showed an even shallower slope. Eye movements showed a gradient response to fine-grained acoustic differences for all listeners. Even considering only trials in which a participant clicked “b” (for example), and accounting for variation in the category boundary, participants made more looks to the competitor (“p”) as the voice onset time neared the boundary. CI users showed a similar pattern, but looked to the competitor more than NH listeners, and this was not different at different continuum steps. CONCLUSION:Residual acoustic hearing did not improve voicing categorization suggesting it may not help identify these phonetic cues. The fact that acoustic + electric users showed poorer performance on fricatives was unexpected as they usually show a benefit in standardized perception measures, and as sibilants contain little energy in the low-frequency (acoustic) range. The authors hypothesize that these listeners may overweight acoustic input, and have problems when this is not available (in fricatives). Thus, the benefit (or cost) of acoustic hearing for phonetic categorization may be complex. Eye movements suggest that in both CI and NH listeners, phoneme categorization is not a process of mapping continuous cues to discrete categories. Rather listeners preserve gradiency as a way to deal with uncertainty. CI listeners appear to adapt to their implant (in part) by amplifying competitor activation to preserve their flexibility in the face of potential misperceptions.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0196-0202</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1538-4667</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000207</identifier><identifier>PMID: 26317298</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Copyright Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved</publisher><subject>Adaptation, Physiological ; Adult ; Aged ; Case-Control Studies ; Cochlear Implantation ; Cochlear Implants ; Deafness - physiopathology ; Deafness - rehabilitation ; Eye Movement Measurements ; Female ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Phonetics ; Speech Perception ; Uncertainty ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Ear and hearing, 2016-01, Vol.37 (1), p.e37-e51</ispartof><rights>Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c3607-5ee4a9525e8d66e46566fa0653d7167b1f64f2401083add81daa06dad05afed03</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>230,314,777,781,882,27905,27906</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26317298$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>McMurray, Bob</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Farris-Trimble, Ashley</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Seedorff, Michael</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rigler, Hannah</creatorcontrib><title>The Effect of Residual Acoustic Hearing and Adaptation to Uncertainty on Speech Perception in Cochlear Implant Users: Evidence From Eye-Tracking</title><title>Ear and hearing</title><addtitle>Ear Hear</addtitle><description>OBJECTIVES:While outcomes with cochlear implants (CIs) are generally good, performance can be fragile. The authors examined two factors that are crucial for good CI performance. First, while there is a clear benefit for adding residual acoustic hearing to CI stimulation (typically in low frequencies), it is unclear whether this contributes directly to phonetic categorization. Thus, the authors examined perception of voicing (which uses low-frequency acoustic cues) and fricative place of articulation (s/∫, which does not) in CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. Second, in speech categorization experiments, CI users typically show shallower identification functions. These are typically interpreted as deriving from noisy encoding of the signal. However, psycholinguistic work suggests shallow slopes may also be a useful way to adapt to uncertainty. The authors thus employed an eye-tracking paradigm to examine this in CI users. DESIGN:Participants were 30 CI users (with a variety of configurations) and 22 age-matched normal hearing (NH) controls. Participants heard tokens from six b/p and six s/∫ continua (eight steps) spanning real words (e.g., beach/peach, sip/ship). Participants selected the picture corresponding to the word they heard from a screen containing four items (a b-, p-, s- and ∫-initial item). Eye movements to each object were monitored as a measure of how strongly they were considering each interpretation in the moments leading up to their final percept. RESULTS:Mouse-click results (analogous to phoneme identification) for voicing showed a shallower slope for CI users than NH listeners, but no differences between CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. For fricatives, CI users also showed a shallower slope, but unexpectedly, acoustic + electric listeners showed an even shallower slope. Eye movements showed a gradient response to fine-grained acoustic differences for all listeners. Even considering only trials in which a participant clicked “b” (for example), and accounting for variation in the category boundary, participants made more looks to the competitor (“p”) as the voice onset time neared the boundary. CI users showed a similar pattern, but looked to the competitor more than NH listeners, and this was not different at different continuum steps. CONCLUSION:Residual acoustic hearing did not improve voicing categorization suggesting it may not help identify these phonetic cues. The fact that acoustic + electric users showed poorer performance on fricatives was unexpected as they usually show a benefit in standardized perception measures, and as sibilants contain little energy in the low-frequency (acoustic) range. The authors hypothesize that these listeners may overweight acoustic input, and have problems when this is not available (in fricatives). Thus, the benefit (or cost) of acoustic hearing for phonetic categorization may be complex. Eye movements suggest that in both CI and NH listeners, phoneme categorization is not a process of mapping continuous cues to discrete categories. Rather listeners preserve gradiency as a way to deal with uncertainty. CI listeners appear to adapt to their implant (in part) by amplifying competitor activation to preserve their flexibility in the face of potential misperceptions.</description><subject>Adaptation, Physiological</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Case-Control Studies</subject><subject>Cochlear Implantation</subject><subject>Cochlear Implants</subject><subject>Deafness - physiopathology</subject><subject>Deafness - rehabilitation</subject><subject>Eye Movement Measurements</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Phonetics</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><subject>Uncertainty</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0196-0202</issn><issn>1538-4667</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2016</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kcFu1DAQhi0EosvCGyDkI5cUO3HshAPSarullSqBYPdsufakMXXiYDut9i14ZNxuqQoHfLE08__f2PMj9JaSY0pa8WG1OzkmT09JxDO0oHXVFIxz8RwtCG15kevlEXoV4w9CaNly9hIdlbyiomybBfq17QFvug50wr7D3yBaMyuHV9rPMVmNz0AFO15hNRq8MmpKKlk_4uTxbtQQkrJj2uNc-T4B6B5_haBhutfYEa-97l0m4PNhcmpMeBchxI94c2MNZD8-DX7Amz0U26D0dR70Gr3olIvw5uFeot3pZrs-Ky6-fD5fry4KXXEiihqAqbYua2gM58B4zXmnCK8rIygXl7TjrCsZoaSplDENNSp3jTKkVh0YUi3RpwN3mi8HMBrGFJSTU7CDCnvplZV_d0bbyyt_I5mgos3UJXr_AAj-5wwxycFGDS5_E_LuJBU1YRVjlGUpO0h18DEG6B7HUCLvwpQ5TPlvmNn27ukTH01_0suC5iC49S7lvV67-RaC7EG51P-f_Rt0Dq2f</recordid><startdate>201601</startdate><enddate>201601</enddate><creator>McMurray, Bob</creator><creator>Farris-Trimble, Ashley</creator><creator>Seedorff, Michael</creator><creator>Rigler, Hannah</creator><general>Copyright Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201601</creationdate><title>The Effect of Residual Acoustic Hearing and Adaptation to Uncertainty on Speech Perception in Cochlear Implant Users: Evidence From Eye-Tracking</title><author>McMurray, Bob ; Farris-Trimble, Ashley ; Seedorff, Michael ; Rigler, Hannah</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c3607-5ee4a9525e8d66e46566fa0653d7167b1f64f2401083add81daa06dad05afed03</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2016</creationdate><topic>Adaptation, Physiological</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Case-Control Studies</topic><topic>Cochlear Implantation</topic><topic>Cochlear Implants</topic><topic>Deafness - physiopathology</topic><topic>Deafness - rehabilitation</topic><topic>Eye Movement Measurements</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Phonetics</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><topic>Uncertainty</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>McMurray, Bob</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Farris-Trimble, Ashley</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Seedorff, Michael</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rigler, Hannah</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Ear and hearing</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>McMurray, Bob</au><au>Farris-Trimble, Ashley</au><au>Seedorff, Michael</au><au>Rigler, Hannah</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The Effect of Residual Acoustic Hearing and Adaptation to Uncertainty on Speech Perception in Cochlear Implant Users: Evidence From Eye-Tracking</atitle><jtitle>Ear and hearing</jtitle><addtitle>Ear Hear</addtitle><date>2016-01</date><risdate>2016</risdate><volume>37</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>e37</spage><epage>e51</epage><pages>e37-e51</pages><issn>0196-0202</issn><eissn>1538-4667</eissn><abstract>OBJECTIVES:While outcomes with cochlear implants (CIs) are generally good, performance can be fragile. The authors examined two factors that are crucial for good CI performance. First, while there is a clear benefit for adding residual acoustic hearing to CI stimulation (typically in low frequencies), it is unclear whether this contributes directly to phonetic categorization. Thus, the authors examined perception of voicing (which uses low-frequency acoustic cues) and fricative place of articulation (s/∫, which does not) in CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. Second, in speech categorization experiments, CI users typically show shallower identification functions. These are typically interpreted as deriving from noisy encoding of the signal. However, psycholinguistic work suggests shallow slopes may also be a useful way to adapt to uncertainty. The authors thus employed an eye-tracking paradigm to examine this in CI users. DESIGN:Participants were 30 CI users (with a variety of configurations) and 22 age-matched normal hearing (NH) controls. Participants heard tokens from six b/p and six s/∫ continua (eight steps) spanning real words (e.g., beach/peach, sip/ship). Participants selected the picture corresponding to the word they heard from a screen containing four items (a b-, p-, s- and ∫-initial item). Eye movements to each object were monitored as a measure of how strongly they were considering each interpretation in the moments leading up to their final percept. RESULTS:Mouse-click results (analogous to phoneme identification) for voicing showed a shallower slope for CI users than NH listeners, but no differences between CI users with and without residual acoustic hearing. For fricatives, CI users also showed a shallower slope, but unexpectedly, acoustic + electric listeners showed an even shallower slope. Eye movements showed a gradient response to fine-grained acoustic differences for all listeners. Even considering only trials in which a participant clicked “b” (for example), and accounting for variation in the category boundary, participants made more looks to the competitor (“p”) as the voice onset time neared the boundary. CI users showed a similar pattern, but looked to the competitor more than NH listeners, and this was not different at different continuum steps. CONCLUSION:Residual acoustic hearing did not improve voicing categorization suggesting it may not help identify these phonetic cues. The fact that acoustic + electric users showed poorer performance on fricatives was unexpected as they usually show a benefit in standardized perception measures, and as sibilants contain little energy in the low-frequency (acoustic) range. The authors hypothesize that these listeners may overweight acoustic input, and have problems when this is not available (in fricatives). Thus, the benefit (or cost) of acoustic hearing for phonetic categorization may be complex. Eye movements suggest that in both CI and NH listeners, phoneme categorization is not a process of mapping continuous cues to discrete categories. Rather listeners preserve gradiency as a way to deal with uncertainty. CI listeners appear to adapt to their implant (in part) by amplifying competitor activation to preserve their flexibility in the face of potential misperceptions.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Copyright Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved</pub><pmid>26317298</pmid><doi>10.1097/AUD.0000000000000207</doi></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0196-0202
ispartof Ear and hearing, 2016-01, Vol.37 (1), p.e37-e51
issn 0196-0202
1538-4667
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4717908
source MEDLINE; Journals@Ovid Complete
subjects Adaptation, Physiological
Adult
Aged
Case-Control Studies
Cochlear Implantation
Cochlear Implants
Deafness - physiopathology
Deafness - rehabilitation
Eye Movement Measurements
Female
Humans
Male
Middle Aged
Phonetics
Speech Perception
Uncertainty
Young Adult
title The Effect of Residual Acoustic Hearing and Adaptation to Uncertainty on Speech Perception in Cochlear Implant Users: Evidence From Eye-Tracking
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-20T05%3A10%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20Effect%20of%20Residual%20Acoustic%20Hearing%20and%20Adaptation%20to%20Uncertainty%20on%20Speech%20Perception%20in%20Cochlear%20Implant%20Users:%20Evidence%20From%20Eye-Tracking&rft.jtitle=Ear%20and%20hearing&rft.au=McMurray,%20Bob&rft.date=2016-01&rft.volume=37&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=e37&rft.epage=e51&rft.pages=e37-e51&rft.issn=0196-0202&rft.eissn=1538-4667&rft_id=info:doi/10.1097/AUD.0000000000000207&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E1750434414%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1750434414&rft_id=info:pmid/26317298&rfr_iscdi=true