Action-effect binding is decreased in motor conversion disorder: Implications for sense of agency

ABSTRACT The abnormal movements seen in motor conversion disorder are affected by distraction and entrainment, similar to voluntary movement. Unlike voluntary movement, however, patients lack a sense of control for the abnormal movements, a failure of “self‐agency.” The action‐effect binding paradig...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Movement disorders 2013-07, Vol.28 (8), p.1110-1116
Hauptverfasser: Kranick, Sarah M., Moore, James W., Yusuf, Nadia, Martinez, Valeria T., LaFaver, Kathrin, Edwards, Mark J., Mehta, Arpan R., Collins, Phoebe, Harrison, Neil A., Haggard, Patrick, Hallett, Mark, Voon, Valerie
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 1116
container_issue 8
container_start_page 1110
container_title Movement disorders
container_volume 28
creator Kranick, Sarah M.
Moore, James W.
Yusuf, Nadia
Martinez, Valeria T.
LaFaver, Kathrin
Edwards, Mark J.
Mehta, Arpan R.
Collins, Phoebe
Harrison, Neil A.
Haggard, Patrick
Hallett, Mark
Voon, Valerie
description ABSTRACT The abnormal movements seen in motor conversion disorder are affected by distraction and entrainment, similar to voluntary movement. Unlike voluntary movement, however, patients lack a sense of control for the abnormal movements, a failure of “self‐agency.” The action‐effect binding paradigm has been used to quantify the sense of self‐agency, because subjective contraction of time between an action and its effect only occurs if the patient feels that they are the agent responsible for the action. We used this paradigm, coupled with emotional stimuli, to investigate the sense of agency with voluntary movements in patients with motor conversion disorder. Twenty patients with motor conversion disorder and 20 age‐matched and sex‐matched healthy volunteers used a rotating clock to judge the time of their own voluntary key presses (action) and a subsequent auditory tone (effect) after they completed conditioning blocks in which high, medium, and low tones were coupled to images of happy, fearful, and neutral faces. The results replicated those produced previously: it was reported that an effect after a voluntary action occurred earlier, and the preceding action occurred later, compared with trials that used only key presses or tones. Patients had reduced overall binding scores relative to healthy volunteers, suggesting a reduced sense of agency. There was no effect of the emotional stimuli (faces) or other interaction effects. Healthy volunteers with subclinical depressive symptoms had higher overall binding scores. We demonstrate that patients with motor conversion disorder have decreased action‐effect binding for normal voluntary movements compared with healthy volunteers, consistent with the greater experience of lack of control. © 2013 Movement Disorder Society
doi_str_mv 10.1002/mds.25408
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3701023</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1891867698</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5808-671da12993308e02c15cb8e7382799eb338b4ba9b184c0e4415d5fa13858f1bf3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0U1vFCEYB3BiNHatHvwChsSLHqaFAQbw0KTZvtikvqRqTLwQhnlmpc7ACrOt--2lbrtRE-OJA7_nH3j-CD2lZI8SUu-PXd6rBSfqHppRwWilaiHvoxlRSlSMKrGDHuV8SQilgjYP0U7NuOZaihmyh27yMVTQ9-Am3PrQ-bDAPuMOXAKbocM-4DFOMWEXwxWkXDzufI6pg_QKn43LwTt7k5JxX1SGkAHHHtsFBLd-jB70dsjw5PbcRZ9Ojj_OX1fn707P5ofnlROKqKqRtLO01poxooDUjgrXKpBM1VJraBlTLW-tbqnijgDnVHSit5QpoXra9mwXHWxyl6t2hM5BmJIdzDL50aa1idabP2-C_2oW8cowSSipWQl4cRuQ4vcV5MmMPjsYBhsgrrKhSlPVyEar_1Nek4aoElro87_oZVylUDZRFNWccCVkUS83yqWYc4J--25KzE3HpnRsfnVc7LPfP7qVd6UWsL8B136A9b-TzJujD3eR1WbC5wl-bCds-mYayaQwn9-emoujk_fzCz43X9hPq8C_zw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1419404857</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Action-effect binding is decreased in motor conversion disorder: Implications for sense of agency</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Wiley Online Library Journals Frontfile Complete</source><creator>Kranick, Sarah M. ; Moore, James W. ; Yusuf, Nadia ; Martinez, Valeria T. ; LaFaver, Kathrin ; Edwards, Mark J. ; Mehta, Arpan R. ; Collins, Phoebe ; Harrison, Neil A. ; Haggard, Patrick ; Hallett, Mark ; Voon, Valerie</creator><creatorcontrib>Kranick, Sarah M. ; Moore, James W. ; Yusuf, Nadia ; Martinez, Valeria T. ; LaFaver, Kathrin ; Edwards, Mark J. ; Mehta, Arpan R. ; Collins, Phoebe ; Harrison, Neil A. ; Haggard, Patrick ; Hallett, Mark ; Voon, Valerie</creatorcontrib><description>ABSTRACT The abnormal movements seen in motor conversion disorder are affected by distraction and entrainment, similar to voluntary movement. Unlike voluntary movement, however, patients lack a sense of control for the abnormal movements, a failure of “self‐agency.” The action‐effect binding paradigm has been used to quantify the sense of self‐agency, because subjective contraction of time between an action and its effect only occurs if the patient feels that they are the agent responsible for the action. We used this paradigm, coupled with emotional stimuli, to investigate the sense of agency with voluntary movements in patients with motor conversion disorder. Twenty patients with motor conversion disorder and 20 age‐matched and sex‐matched healthy volunteers used a rotating clock to judge the time of their own voluntary key presses (action) and a subsequent auditory tone (effect) after they completed conditioning blocks in which high, medium, and low tones were coupled to images of happy, fearful, and neutral faces. The results replicated those produced previously: it was reported that an effect after a voluntary action occurred earlier, and the preceding action occurred later, compared with trials that used only key presses or tones. Patients had reduced overall binding scores relative to healthy volunteers, suggesting a reduced sense of agency. There was no effect of the emotional stimuli (faces) or other interaction effects. Healthy volunteers with subclinical depressive symptoms had higher overall binding scores. We demonstrate that patients with motor conversion disorder have decreased action‐effect binding for normal voluntary movements compared with healthy volunteers, consistent with the greater experience of lack of control. © 2013 Movement Disorder Society</description><identifier>ISSN: 0885-3185</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1531-8257</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1002/mds.25408</identifier><identifier>PMID: 23494975</identifier><identifier>CODEN: MOVDEA</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>action-effect binding ; Adult ; Aged ; agency ; Awareness ; Case-Control Studies ; conversion disorder ; Conversion Disorder - complications ; Conversion Disorder - psychology ; Cooperative Behavior ; Dyskinesias ; Female ; forward model ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Mood Disorders - etiology ; Movement disorders ; psychogenic movement disorder ; Psychomotor Performance ; Regression Analysis ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Movement disorders, 2013-07, Vol.28 (8), p.1110-1116</ispartof><rights>Copyright © 2013 Movement Disorder Society</rights><rights>Copyright © 2013 Movement Disorder Society.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5808-671da12993308e02c15cb8e7382799eb338b4ba9b184c0e4415d5fa13858f1bf3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5808-671da12993308e02c15cb8e7382799eb338b4ba9b184c0e4415d5fa13858f1bf3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002%2Fmds.25408$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002%2Fmds.25408$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,776,780,881,1411,27901,27902,45550,45551</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23494975$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Kranick, Sarah M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Moore, James W.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Yusuf, Nadia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Martinez, Valeria T.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>LaFaver, Kathrin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Edwards, Mark J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mehta, Arpan R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Collins, Phoebe</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Harrison, Neil A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Haggard, Patrick</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hallett, Mark</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Voon, Valerie</creatorcontrib><title>Action-effect binding is decreased in motor conversion disorder: Implications for sense of agency</title><title>Movement disorders</title><addtitle>Mov Disord</addtitle><description>ABSTRACT The abnormal movements seen in motor conversion disorder are affected by distraction and entrainment, similar to voluntary movement. Unlike voluntary movement, however, patients lack a sense of control for the abnormal movements, a failure of “self‐agency.” The action‐effect binding paradigm has been used to quantify the sense of self‐agency, because subjective contraction of time between an action and its effect only occurs if the patient feels that they are the agent responsible for the action. We used this paradigm, coupled with emotional stimuli, to investigate the sense of agency with voluntary movements in patients with motor conversion disorder. Twenty patients with motor conversion disorder and 20 age‐matched and sex‐matched healthy volunteers used a rotating clock to judge the time of their own voluntary key presses (action) and a subsequent auditory tone (effect) after they completed conditioning blocks in which high, medium, and low tones were coupled to images of happy, fearful, and neutral faces. The results replicated those produced previously: it was reported that an effect after a voluntary action occurred earlier, and the preceding action occurred later, compared with trials that used only key presses or tones. Patients had reduced overall binding scores relative to healthy volunteers, suggesting a reduced sense of agency. There was no effect of the emotional stimuli (faces) or other interaction effects. Healthy volunteers with subclinical depressive symptoms had higher overall binding scores. We demonstrate that patients with motor conversion disorder have decreased action‐effect binding for normal voluntary movements compared with healthy volunteers, consistent with the greater experience of lack of control. © 2013 Movement Disorder Society</description><subject>action-effect binding</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Aged</subject><subject>agency</subject><subject>Awareness</subject><subject>Case-Control Studies</subject><subject>conversion disorder</subject><subject>Conversion Disorder - complications</subject><subject>Conversion Disorder - psychology</subject><subject>Cooperative Behavior</subject><subject>Dyskinesias</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>forward model</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Mood Disorders - etiology</subject><subject>Movement disorders</subject><subject>psychogenic movement disorder</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance</subject><subject>Regression Analysis</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0885-3185</issn><issn>1531-8257</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2013</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0U1vFCEYB3BiNHatHvwChsSLHqaFAQbw0KTZvtikvqRqTLwQhnlmpc7ACrOt--2lbrtRE-OJA7_nH3j-CD2lZI8SUu-PXd6rBSfqHppRwWilaiHvoxlRSlSMKrGDHuV8SQilgjYP0U7NuOZaihmyh27yMVTQ9-Am3PrQ-bDAPuMOXAKbocM-4DFOMWEXwxWkXDzufI6pg_QKn43LwTt7k5JxX1SGkAHHHtsFBLd-jB70dsjw5PbcRZ9Ojj_OX1fn707P5ofnlROKqKqRtLO01poxooDUjgrXKpBM1VJraBlTLW-tbqnijgDnVHSit5QpoXra9mwXHWxyl6t2hM5BmJIdzDL50aa1idabP2-C_2oW8cowSSipWQl4cRuQ4vcV5MmMPjsYBhsgrrKhSlPVyEar_1Nek4aoElro87_oZVylUDZRFNWccCVkUS83yqWYc4J--25KzE3HpnRsfnVc7LPfP7qVd6UWsL8B136A9b-TzJujD3eR1WbC5wl-bCds-mYayaQwn9-emoujk_fzCz43X9hPq8C_zw</recordid><startdate>201307</startdate><enddate>201307</enddate><creator>Kranick, Sarah M.</creator><creator>Moore, James W.</creator><creator>Yusuf, Nadia</creator><creator>Martinez, Valeria T.</creator><creator>LaFaver, Kathrin</creator><creator>Edwards, Mark J.</creator><creator>Mehta, Arpan R.</creator><creator>Collins, Phoebe</creator><creator>Harrison, Neil A.</creator><creator>Haggard, Patrick</creator><creator>Hallett, Mark</creator><creator>Voon, Valerie</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>201307</creationdate><title>Action-effect binding is decreased in motor conversion disorder: Implications for sense of agency</title><author>Kranick, Sarah M. ; Moore, James W. ; Yusuf, Nadia ; Martinez, Valeria T. ; LaFaver, Kathrin ; Edwards, Mark J. ; Mehta, Arpan R. ; Collins, Phoebe ; Harrison, Neil A. ; Haggard, Patrick ; Hallett, Mark ; Voon, Valerie</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5808-671da12993308e02c15cb8e7382799eb338b4ba9b184c0e4415d5fa13858f1bf3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2013</creationdate><topic>action-effect binding</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Aged</topic><topic>agency</topic><topic>Awareness</topic><topic>Case-Control Studies</topic><topic>conversion disorder</topic><topic>Conversion Disorder - complications</topic><topic>Conversion Disorder - psychology</topic><topic>Cooperative Behavior</topic><topic>Dyskinesias</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>forward model</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Mood Disorders - etiology</topic><topic>Movement disorders</topic><topic>psychogenic movement disorder</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance</topic><topic>Regression Analysis</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Kranick, Sarah M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Moore, James W.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Yusuf, Nadia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Martinez, Valeria T.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>LaFaver, Kathrin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Edwards, Mark J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mehta, Arpan R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Collins, Phoebe</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Harrison, Neil A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Haggard, Patrick</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hallett, Mark</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Voon, Valerie</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing &amp; Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Movement disorders</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Kranick, Sarah M.</au><au>Moore, James W.</au><au>Yusuf, Nadia</au><au>Martinez, Valeria T.</au><au>LaFaver, Kathrin</au><au>Edwards, Mark J.</au><au>Mehta, Arpan R.</au><au>Collins, Phoebe</au><au>Harrison, Neil A.</au><au>Haggard, Patrick</au><au>Hallett, Mark</au><au>Voon, Valerie</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Action-effect binding is decreased in motor conversion disorder: Implications for sense of agency</atitle><jtitle>Movement disorders</jtitle><addtitle>Mov Disord</addtitle><date>2013-07</date><risdate>2013</risdate><volume>28</volume><issue>8</issue><spage>1110</spage><epage>1116</epage><pages>1110-1116</pages><issn>0885-3185</issn><eissn>1531-8257</eissn><coden>MOVDEA</coden><abstract>ABSTRACT The abnormal movements seen in motor conversion disorder are affected by distraction and entrainment, similar to voluntary movement. Unlike voluntary movement, however, patients lack a sense of control for the abnormal movements, a failure of “self‐agency.” The action‐effect binding paradigm has been used to quantify the sense of self‐agency, because subjective contraction of time between an action and its effect only occurs if the patient feels that they are the agent responsible for the action. We used this paradigm, coupled with emotional stimuli, to investigate the sense of agency with voluntary movements in patients with motor conversion disorder. Twenty patients with motor conversion disorder and 20 age‐matched and sex‐matched healthy volunteers used a rotating clock to judge the time of their own voluntary key presses (action) and a subsequent auditory tone (effect) after they completed conditioning blocks in which high, medium, and low tones were coupled to images of happy, fearful, and neutral faces. The results replicated those produced previously: it was reported that an effect after a voluntary action occurred earlier, and the preceding action occurred later, compared with trials that used only key presses or tones. Patients had reduced overall binding scores relative to healthy volunteers, suggesting a reduced sense of agency. There was no effect of the emotional stimuli (faces) or other interaction effects. Healthy volunteers with subclinical depressive symptoms had higher overall binding scores. We demonstrate that patients with motor conversion disorder have decreased action‐effect binding for normal voluntary movements compared with healthy volunteers, consistent with the greater experience of lack of control. © 2013 Movement Disorder Society</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><pmid>23494975</pmid><doi>10.1002/mds.25408</doi><tpages>7</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0885-3185
ispartof Movement disorders, 2013-07, Vol.28 (8), p.1110-1116
issn 0885-3185
1531-8257
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3701023
source MEDLINE; Wiley Online Library Journals Frontfile Complete
subjects action-effect binding
Adult
Aged
agency
Awareness
Case-Control Studies
conversion disorder
Conversion Disorder - complications
Conversion Disorder - psychology
Cooperative Behavior
Dyskinesias
Female
forward model
Humans
Male
Middle Aged
Mood Disorders - etiology
Movement disorders
psychogenic movement disorder
Psychomotor Performance
Regression Analysis
Young Adult
title Action-effect binding is decreased in motor conversion disorder: Implications for sense of agency
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-07T09%3A04%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Action-effect%20binding%20is%20decreased%20in%20motor%20conversion%20disorder:%20Implications%20for%20sense%20of%20agency&rft.jtitle=Movement%20disorders&rft.au=Kranick,%20Sarah%20M.&rft.date=2013-07&rft.volume=28&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=1110&rft.epage=1116&rft.pages=1110-1116&rft.issn=0885-3185&rft.eissn=1531-8257&rft.coden=MOVDEA&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002/mds.25408&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E1891867698%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1419404857&rft_id=info:pmid/23494975&rfr_iscdi=true