Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues
The correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Cognition 2009-11, Vol.113 (2), p.150-166 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 166 |
---|---|
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 150 |
container_title | Cognition |
container_volume | 113 |
creator | Hollingworth, Andrew Franconeri, Steven L. |
description | The
correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2764005</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ859198</ericid><els_id>S001002770900184X</els_id><sourcerecordid>37236044</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-7d34f60b8574df4a59745293f7afefa4658a5f5a20900f9fae4e89de222be863</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkktvEzEUhUcIREPhHyDwBlglXL_G9qZSVZWXKnXTveXxXDeOJuNgz1Tw73FIFGBDV5Z8vnt8rHua5i2FFQXaftysfLof4xTTuGIAZgV6BSCeNAuqFV8qzfXTZgFAYQlMqbPmRSkbqART-nlzRo1ihkq5aH7cdhv0E_EpZyy7NPY4eiTO51QK6XLEQJL3w1zqUyQWgmVy3RDLGntSb6Y1ks6VKqRAujStSdm5GmvC7S5lNxA39qTMObjqGtBNc0biZywvm2fBDQVfHc_z5u7T9d3Vl-XN7eevV5c3S99KPi1Vz0VoodNSiT4IJ40SkhkelAsYnGildjJIx8AABBMcCtSmR8ZYh7rl583FwXY3d1vsPY5TTWV3OW5d_mmTi_ZfZYxre58eLFOtAJDV4MPRIKfvNfdkt7F4HAY3YpqLVVyAoS2YSr7_L8kV4y0I8SjIKJiW8r2jOoC_t5ExnHJTsPse2I099cDue2BB27rlOvnm72__mTsuvgLvjoAr3g0hu9HHcuJYzcC12nOvDxzm6E_y9TctDTW6ypdHua7wIWK2xcd9g_qYa61sn-KjWX8BS9jiMQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>21096139</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Hollingworth, Andrew ; Franconeri, Steven L.</creator><creatorcontrib>Hollingworth, Andrew ; Franconeri, Steven L.</creatorcontrib><description>The
correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7838</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19729155</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CGTNAU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Barriers ; Biological and medical sciences ; Cognition ; Cognitive Development ; Color ; Color Perception - physiology ; Concept Formation - physiology ; Correlation ; Cues ; Dynamic vision ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Geometric Concepts ; Humans ; Male ; Mind ; Object ; Object correspondence ; Object perception ; Object persistence ; Occlusion ; Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology ; Perception ; Photic Stimulation ; Problem Solving - physiology ; Psychology ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Sight ; Space ; Space Perception - physiology ; Spatial Ability ; Time ; Vision ; Visual Perception</subject><ispartof>Cognition, 2009-11, Vol.113 (2), p.150-166</ispartof><rights>2009 Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-7d34f60b8574df4a59745293f7afefa4658a5f5a20900f9fae4e89de222be863</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,778,782,883,3539,27907,27908,45978</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ859198$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=22103875$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19729155$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hollingworth, Andrew</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Franconeri, Steven L.</creatorcontrib><title>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</title><title>Cognition</title><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><description>The
correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Barriers</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive Development</subject><subject>Color</subject><subject>Color Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Concept Formation - physiology</subject><subject>Correlation</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Dynamic vision</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Geometric Concepts</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind</subject><subject>Object</subject><subject>Object correspondence</subject><subject>Object perception</subject><subject>Object persistence</subject><subject>Occlusion</subject><subject>Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Problem Solving - physiology</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Sight</subject><subject>Space</subject><subject>Space Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Spatial Ability</subject><subject>Time</subject><subject>Vision</subject><subject>Visual Perception</subject><issn>0010-0277</issn><issn>1873-7838</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2009</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkktvEzEUhUcIREPhHyDwBlglXL_G9qZSVZWXKnXTveXxXDeOJuNgz1Tw73FIFGBDV5Z8vnt8rHua5i2FFQXaftysfLof4xTTuGIAZgV6BSCeNAuqFV8qzfXTZgFAYQlMqbPmRSkbqART-nlzRo1ihkq5aH7cdhv0E_EpZyy7NPY4eiTO51QK6XLEQJL3w1zqUyQWgmVy3RDLGntSb6Y1ks6VKqRAujStSdm5GmvC7S5lNxA39qTMObjqGtBNc0biZywvm2fBDQVfHc_z5u7T9d3Vl-XN7eevV5c3S99KPi1Vz0VoodNSiT4IJ40SkhkelAsYnGildjJIx8AABBMcCtSmR8ZYh7rl583FwXY3d1vsPY5TTWV3OW5d_mmTi_ZfZYxre58eLFOtAJDV4MPRIKfvNfdkt7F4HAY3YpqLVVyAoS2YSr7_L8kV4y0I8SjIKJiW8r2jOoC_t5ExnHJTsPse2I099cDue2BB27rlOvnm72__mTsuvgLvjoAr3g0hu9HHcuJYzcC12nOvDxzm6E_y9TctDTW6ypdHua7wIWK2xcd9g_qYa61sn-KjWX8BS9jiMQ</recordid><startdate>20091101</startdate><enddate>20091101</enddate><creator>Hollingworth, Andrew</creator><creator>Franconeri, Steven L.</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20091101</creationdate><title>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</title><author>Hollingworth, Andrew ; Franconeri, Steven L.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-7d34f60b8574df4a59745293f7afefa4658a5f5a20900f9fae4e89de222be863</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2009</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Barriers</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive Development</topic><topic>Color</topic><topic>Color Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Concept Formation - physiology</topic><topic>Correlation</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Dynamic vision</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Geometric Concepts</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind</topic><topic>Object</topic><topic>Object correspondence</topic><topic>Object perception</topic><topic>Object persistence</topic><topic>Occlusion</topic><topic>Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Problem Solving - physiology</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Sight</topic><topic>Space</topic><topic>Space Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Spatial Ability</topic><topic>Time</topic><topic>Vision</topic><topic>Visual Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hollingworth, Andrew</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Franconeri, Steven L.</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hollingworth, Andrew</au><au>Franconeri, Steven L.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ859198</ericid><atitle>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</atitle><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><date>2009-11-01</date><risdate>2009</risdate><volume>113</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>150</spage><epage>166</epage><pages>150-166</pages><issn>0010-0277</issn><eissn>1873-7838</eissn><coden>CGTNAU</coden><abstract>The
correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>19729155</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004</doi><tpages>17</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0010-0277 |
ispartof | Cognition, 2009-11, Vol.113 (2), p.150-166 |
issn | 0010-0277 1873-7838 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2764005 |
source | MEDLINE; Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Barriers Biological and medical sciences Cognition Cognitive Development Color Color Perception - physiology Concept Formation - physiology Correlation Cues Dynamic vision Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Geometric Concepts Humans Male Mind Object Object correspondence Object perception Object persistence Occlusion Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology Perception Photic Stimulation Problem Solving - physiology Psychology Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor Performance - physiology Reaction Time - physiology Sight Space Space Perception - physiology Spatial Ability Time Vision Visual Perception |
title | Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-17T07%3A03%3A29IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Object%20correspondence%20across%20brief%20occlusion%20is%20established%20on%20the%20basis%20of%20both%20spatiotemporal%20and%20surface%20feature%20cues&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Hollingworth,%20Andrew&rft.date=2009-11-01&rft.volume=113&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=150&rft.epage=166&rft.pages=150-166&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.coden=CGTNAU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E37236044%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=21096139&rft_id=info:pmid/19729155&rft_ericid=EJ859198&rft_els_id=S001002770900184X&rfr_iscdi=true |