Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues

The correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Cognition 2009-11, Vol.113 (2), p.150-166
Hauptverfasser: Hollingworth, Andrew, Franconeri, Steven L.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 166
container_issue 2
container_start_page 150
container_title Cognition
container_volume 113
creator Hollingworth, Andrew
Franconeri, Steven L.
description The correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2764005</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ859198</ericid><els_id>S001002770900184X</els_id><sourcerecordid>37236044</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-7d34f60b8574df4a59745293f7afefa4658a5f5a20900f9fae4e89de222be863</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkktvEzEUhUcIREPhHyDwBlglXL_G9qZSVZWXKnXTveXxXDeOJuNgz1Tw73FIFGBDV5Z8vnt8rHua5i2FFQXaftysfLof4xTTuGIAZgV6BSCeNAuqFV8qzfXTZgFAYQlMqbPmRSkbqART-nlzRo1ihkq5aH7cdhv0E_EpZyy7NPY4eiTO51QK6XLEQJL3w1zqUyQWgmVy3RDLGntSb6Y1ks6VKqRAujStSdm5GmvC7S5lNxA39qTMObjqGtBNc0biZywvm2fBDQVfHc_z5u7T9d3Vl-XN7eevV5c3S99KPi1Vz0VoodNSiT4IJ40SkhkelAsYnGildjJIx8AABBMcCtSmR8ZYh7rl583FwXY3d1vsPY5TTWV3OW5d_mmTi_ZfZYxre58eLFOtAJDV4MPRIKfvNfdkt7F4HAY3YpqLVVyAoS2YSr7_L8kV4y0I8SjIKJiW8r2jOoC_t5ExnHJTsPse2I099cDue2BB27rlOvnm72__mTsuvgLvjoAr3g0hu9HHcuJYzcC12nOvDxzm6E_y9TctDTW6ypdHua7wIWK2xcd9g_qYa61sn-KjWX8BS9jiMQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>21096139</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Hollingworth, Andrew ; Franconeri, Steven L.</creator><creatorcontrib>Hollingworth, Andrew ; Franconeri, Steven L.</creatorcontrib><description>The correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7838</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19729155</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CGTNAU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Barriers ; Biological and medical sciences ; Cognition ; Cognitive Development ; Color ; Color Perception - physiology ; Concept Formation - physiology ; Correlation ; Cues ; Dynamic vision ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Geometric Concepts ; Humans ; Male ; Mind ; Object ; Object correspondence ; Object perception ; Object persistence ; Occlusion ; Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology ; Perception ; Photic Stimulation ; Problem Solving - physiology ; Psychology ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Sight ; Space ; Space Perception - physiology ; Spatial Ability ; Time ; Vision ; Visual Perception</subject><ispartof>Cognition, 2009-11, Vol.113 (2), p.150-166</ispartof><rights>2009 Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-7d34f60b8574df4a59745293f7afefa4658a5f5a20900f9fae4e89de222be863</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,778,782,883,3539,27907,27908,45978</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ859198$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=22103875$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19729155$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hollingworth, Andrew</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Franconeri, Steven L.</creatorcontrib><title>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</title><title>Cognition</title><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><description>The correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Barriers</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognitive Development</subject><subject>Color</subject><subject>Color Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Concept Formation - physiology</subject><subject>Correlation</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Dynamic vision</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Geometric Concepts</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind</subject><subject>Object</subject><subject>Object correspondence</subject><subject>Object perception</subject><subject>Object persistence</subject><subject>Occlusion</subject><subject>Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Problem Solving - physiology</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Sight</subject><subject>Space</subject><subject>Space Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Spatial Ability</subject><subject>Time</subject><subject>Vision</subject><subject>Visual Perception</subject><issn>0010-0277</issn><issn>1873-7838</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2009</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkktvEzEUhUcIREPhHyDwBlglXL_G9qZSVZWXKnXTveXxXDeOJuNgz1Tw73FIFGBDV5Z8vnt8rHua5i2FFQXaftysfLof4xTTuGIAZgV6BSCeNAuqFV8qzfXTZgFAYQlMqbPmRSkbqART-nlzRo1ihkq5aH7cdhv0E_EpZyy7NPY4eiTO51QK6XLEQJL3w1zqUyQWgmVy3RDLGntSb6Y1ks6VKqRAujStSdm5GmvC7S5lNxA39qTMObjqGtBNc0biZywvm2fBDQVfHc_z5u7T9d3Vl-XN7eevV5c3S99KPi1Vz0VoodNSiT4IJ40SkhkelAsYnGildjJIx8AABBMcCtSmR8ZYh7rl583FwXY3d1vsPY5TTWV3OW5d_mmTi_ZfZYxre58eLFOtAJDV4MPRIKfvNfdkt7F4HAY3YpqLVVyAoS2YSr7_L8kV4y0I8SjIKJiW8r2jOoC_t5ExnHJTsPse2I099cDue2BB27rlOvnm72__mTsuvgLvjoAr3g0hu9HHcuJYzcC12nOvDxzm6E_y9TctDTW6ypdHua7wIWK2xcd9g_qYa61sn-KjWX8BS9jiMQ</recordid><startdate>20091101</startdate><enddate>20091101</enddate><creator>Hollingworth, Andrew</creator><creator>Franconeri, Steven L.</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20091101</creationdate><title>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</title><author>Hollingworth, Andrew ; Franconeri, Steven L.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c653t-7d34f60b8574df4a59745293f7afefa4658a5f5a20900f9fae4e89de222be863</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2009</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Barriers</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognitive Development</topic><topic>Color</topic><topic>Color Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Concept Formation - physiology</topic><topic>Correlation</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Dynamic vision</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Geometric Concepts</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind</topic><topic>Object</topic><topic>Object correspondence</topic><topic>Object perception</topic><topic>Object persistence</topic><topic>Occlusion</topic><topic>Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Problem Solving - physiology</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Sight</topic><topic>Space</topic><topic>Space Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Spatial Ability</topic><topic>Time</topic><topic>Vision</topic><topic>Visual Perception</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hollingworth, Andrew</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Franconeri, Steven L.</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hollingworth, Andrew</au><au>Franconeri, Steven L.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ859198</ericid><atitle>Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues</atitle><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><date>2009-11-01</date><risdate>2009</risdate><volume>113</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>150</spage><epage>166</epage><pages>150-166</pages><issn>0010-0277</issn><eissn>1873-7838</eissn><coden>CGTNAU</coden><abstract>The correspondence problem is a classic issue in vision and cognition. Frequent perceptual disruptions, such as saccades and brief occlusion, create gaps in perceptual input. How does the visual system establish correspondence between objects visible before and after the disruption? Current theories hold that object correspondence is established solely on the basis of an object’s spatiotemporal properties and that an object’s surface feature properties (such as color or shape) are not consulted in correspondence operations. In five experiments, we tested the relative contributions of spatiotemporal and surface feature properties to establishing object correspondence across brief occlusion. Correspondence operations were strongly influenced both by the consistency of an object’s spatiotemporal properties across occlusion and by the consistency of an object’s surface feature properties across occlusion. These data argue against the claim that spatiotemporal cues dominate the computation of object correspondence. Instead, the visual system consults multiple sources of relevant information to establish continuity across perceptual disruption.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>19729155</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004</doi><tpages>17</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0010-0277
ispartof Cognition, 2009-11, Vol.113 (2), p.150-166
issn 0010-0277
1873-7838
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2764005
source MEDLINE; Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Adolescent
Adult
Barriers
Biological and medical sciences
Cognition
Cognitive Development
Color
Color Perception - physiology
Concept Formation - physiology
Correlation
Cues
Dynamic vision
Female
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Geometric Concepts
Humans
Male
Mind
Object
Object correspondence
Object perception
Object persistence
Occlusion
Pattern Recognition, Visual - physiology
Perception
Photic Stimulation
Problem Solving - physiology
Psychology
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Reaction Time - physiology
Sight
Space
Space Perception - physiology
Spatial Ability
Time
Vision
Visual Perception
title Object correspondence across brief occlusion is established on the basis of both spatiotemporal and surface feature cues
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-17T07%3A03%3A29IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Object%20correspondence%20across%20brief%20occlusion%20is%20established%20on%20the%20basis%20of%20both%20spatiotemporal%20and%20surface%20feature%20cues&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Hollingworth,%20Andrew&rft.date=2009-11-01&rft.volume=113&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=150&rft.epage=166&rft.pages=150-166&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.coden=CGTNAU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.004&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E37236044%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=21096139&rft_id=info:pmid/19729155&rft_ericid=EJ859198&rft_els_id=S001002770900184X&rfr_iscdi=true