Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages
When natural language input contains grammatical forms that are used probabilistically and inconsistently, learners will sometimes reproduce the inconsistencies; but sometimes they will instead regularize the use of these forms, introducing consistency in the language that was not present in the inp...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Cognitive psychology 2009-08, Vol.59 (1), p.30-66 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 66 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 30 |
container_title | Cognitive psychology |
container_volume | 59 |
creator | Hudson Kam, Carla L. Newport, Elissa L. |
description | When natural language input contains grammatical forms that are used probabilistically and inconsistently, learners will sometimes reproduce the inconsistencies; but sometimes they will instead regularize the use of these forms, introducing consistency in the language that was not present in the input. In this paper we ask what produces such regularization. We conducted three artificial language experiments, varying the use of determiners in the types of inconsistency with which they are used, and also comparing adult and child learners. In Experiment 1 we presented adult learners with
scattered inconsistency – the use of multiple determiners varying in frequency in the same context – and found that adults will reproduce these inconsistencies at low levels of scatter, but at very high levels of scatter will regularize the determiner system, producing the most frequent determiner form almost all the time. In Experiment 2 we showed that this is not merely the result of frequency: when determiners are used with low frequencies but in consistent contexts, adults will learn all of the determiners veridically. In Experiment 3 we compared adult and child learners, finding that children will almost always regularize inconsistent forms, whereas adult learners will only regularize the most complex inconsistencies. Taken together, these results suggest that regularization processes in natural language learning, such as those seen in the acquisition of language from non-native speakers or in the formation of young languages, may depend crucially on the nature of language learning by young children. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.01.001 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2703698</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ845728</ericid><els_id>S0010028509000048</els_id><sourcerecordid>1768388591</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c640t-29229ea11547b70adb03ba86486e3cfdb8d4563337a54a80706e28c1b4e8549e3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkk1vEzEQhlcIREPhH6BqhQS3DeNvLwcEKm0BVeIC4mh5vZONo4032Jui_Hu8SpoCl1xsaeaZ1zPjtyguCMwJEPl2NXdDt0k7t5xTgHoOZA5AHhUzArWohKTscTHLEaiAanFWPEtpBQBUSvG0OCM1o5wxOis-3eA4-tCVfiyj75Zj2ezK7iH2Ow6he1f-XGIoe7QxYEylW9rQYdnnc2s7TM-LJwvbJ3xxuM-LH9dX3y8_V7ffbr5cfrytnOQwVrSmtEZLiOCqUWDbBlhjteRaInOLttEtF5IxpqzgVoMCiVQ70nDUgtfIzov3e93Ntllj6zCM0fZmE_3axp0ZrDf_ZoJfmm64M1QBk7XOAm8OAnH4tcU0mrVPDvs8CQ7bZLTIIIA4CUrFKeNcngSFYiCZqk-C-Re1YmJ6-tV_4GrYxpD3aihhkhHFp0HkHnJxSCni4rgEAmYyiFmZe4NM0rUBYrIdcuHF3yt8KDs4IgOvD4BNzvaLaIPz6chRoihTmmfu5Z7D6N0xffVVc6Ho1OCHQzr74c5jNMl5DA5bH9GNph38qVb_AJHp5HE</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>213631748</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Hudson Kam, Carla L. ; Newport, Elissa L.</creator><creatorcontrib>Hudson Kam, Carla L. ; Newport, Elissa L.</creatorcontrib><description>When natural language input contains grammatical forms that are used probabilistically and inconsistently, learners will sometimes reproduce the inconsistencies; but sometimes they will instead regularize the use of these forms, introducing consistency in the language that was not present in the input. In this paper we ask what produces such regularization. We conducted three artificial language experiments, varying the use of determiners in the types of inconsistency with which they are used, and also comparing adult and child learners. In Experiment 1 we presented adult learners with
scattered inconsistency – the use of multiple determiners varying in frequency in the same context – and found that adults will reproduce these inconsistencies at low levels of scatter, but at very high levels of scatter will regularize the determiner system, producing the most frequent determiner form almost all the time. In Experiment 2 we showed that this is not merely the result of frequency: when determiners are used with low frequencies but in consistent contexts, adults will learn all of the determiners veridically. In Experiment 3 we compared adult and child learners, finding that children will almost always regularize inconsistent forms, whereas adult learners will only regularize the most complex inconsistencies. Taken together, these results suggest that regularization processes in natural language learning, such as those seen in the acquisition of language from non-native speakers or in the formation of young languages, may depend crucially on the nature of language learning by young children.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0285</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1095-5623</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.01.001</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19324332</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CGPSBQ</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Adult ; Adult Basic Education ; Adult Learning ; Adult Students ; Adults ; Age Differences ; Age Factors ; Artificial Languages ; Bilingualism. Multilingualism ; Biological and medical sciences ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Children & youth ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognitive psychology ; Comparative Analysis ; English (Second Language) ; Error Analysis (Language) ; Experimental psychology ; Experiments ; Female ; Form Classes (Languages) ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Grammar ; Humans ; Inconsistency ; Language ; Language Acquisition ; Language Development ; Learning ; Linguistic Input ; Male ; Miniature artificial languages ; Multilingualism ; Native Speakers ; Natural Language Processing ; Probability learning ; Psycholinguistics ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Regularization ; Second language learning ; Young Children</subject><ispartof>Cognitive psychology, 2009-08, Vol.59 (1), p.30-66</ispartof><rights>2009 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>2009 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright Academic Press Aug 2009</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c640t-29229ea11547b70adb03ba86486e3cfdb8d4563337a54a80706e28c1b4e8549e3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c640t-29229ea11547b70adb03ba86486e3cfdb8d4563337a54a80706e28c1b4e8549e3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.01.001$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,315,782,786,887,3552,27931,27932,31006,31007,46002</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ845728$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=21723784$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19324332$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hudson Kam, Carla L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Newport, Elissa L.</creatorcontrib><title>Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages</title><title>Cognitive psychology</title><addtitle>Cogn Psychol</addtitle><description>When natural language input contains grammatical forms that are used probabilistically and inconsistently, learners will sometimes reproduce the inconsistencies; but sometimes they will instead regularize the use of these forms, introducing consistency in the language that was not present in the input. In this paper we ask what produces such regularization. We conducted three artificial language experiments, varying the use of determiners in the types of inconsistency with which they are used, and also comparing adult and child learners. In Experiment 1 we presented adult learners with
scattered inconsistency – the use of multiple determiners varying in frequency in the same context – and found that adults will reproduce these inconsistencies at low levels of scatter, but at very high levels of scatter will regularize the determiner system, producing the most frequent determiner form almost all the time. In Experiment 2 we showed that this is not merely the result of frequency: when determiners are used with low frequencies but in consistent contexts, adults will learn all of the determiners veridically. In Experiment 3 we compared adult and child learners, finding that children will almost always regularize inconsistent forms, whereas adult learners will only regularize the most complex inconsistencies. Taken together, these results suggest that regularization processes in natural language learning, such as those seen in the acquisition of language from non-native speakers or in the formation of young languages, may depend crucially on the nature of language learning by young children.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Adult Basic Education</subject><subject>Adult Learning</subject><subject>Adult Students</subject><subject>Adults</subject><subject>Age Differences</subject><subject>Age Factors</subject><subject>Artificial Languages</subject><subject>Bilingualism. Multilingualism</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children & youth</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Cognitive psychology</subject><subject>Comparative Analysis</subject><subject>English (Second Language)</subject><subject>Error Analysis (Language)</subject><subject>Experimental psychology</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Form Classes (Languages)</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Grammar</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Inconsistency</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language Acquisition</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Linguistic Input</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Miniature artificial languages</subject><subject>Multilingualism</subject><subject>Native Speakers</subject><subject>Natural Language Processing</subject><subject>Probability learning</subject><subject>Psycholinguistics</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Regularization</subject><subject>Second language learning</subject><subject>Young Children</subject><issn>0010-0285</issn><issn>1095-5623</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2009</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkk1vEzEQhlcIREPhH6BqhQS3DeNvLwcEKm0BVeIC4mh5vZONo4032Jui_Hu8SpoCl1xsaeaZ1zPjtyguCMwJEPl2NXdDt0k7t5xTgHoOZA5AHhUzArWohKTscTHLEaiAanFWPEtpBQBUSvG0OCM1o5wxOis-3eA4-tCVfiyj75Zj2ezK7iH2Ow6he1f-XGIoe7QxYEylW9rQYdnnc2s7TM-LJwvbJ3xxuM-LH9dX3y8_V7ffbr5cfrytnOQwVrSmtEZLiOCqUWDbBlhjteRaInOLttEtF5IxpqzgVoMCiVQ70nDUgtfIzov3e93Ntllj6zCM0fZmE_3axp0ZrDf_ZoJfmm64M1QBk7XOAm8OAnH4tcU0mrVPDvs8CQ7bZLTIIIA4CUrFKeNcngSFYiCZqk-C-Re1YmJ6-tV_4GrYxpD3aihhkhHFp0HkHnJxSCni4rgEAmYyiFmZe4NM0rUBYrIdcuHF3yt8KDs4IgOvD4BNzvaLaIPz6chRoihTmmfu5Z7D6N0xffVVc6Ho1OCHQzr74c5jNMl5DA5bH9GNph38qVb_AJHp5HE</recordid><startdate>20090801</startdate><enddate>20090801</enddate><creator>Hudson Kam, Carla L.</creator><creator>Newport, Elissa L.</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier</general><general>Academic Press</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20090801</creationdate><title>Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages</title><author>Hudson Kam, Carla L. ; Newport, Elissa L.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c640t-29229ea11547b70adb03ba86486e3cfdb8d4563337a54a80706e28c1b4e8549e3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2009</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Adult Basic Education</topic><topic>Adult Learning</topic><topic>Adult Students</topic><topic>Adults</topic><topic>Age Differences</topic><topic>Age Factors</topic><topic>Artificial Languages</topic><topic>Bilingualism. Multilingualism</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children & youth</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Cognitive psychology</topic><topic>Comparative Analysis</topic><topic>English (Second Language)</topic><topic>Error Analysis (Language)</topic><topic>Experimental psychology</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Form Classes (Languages)</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Grammar</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Inconsistency</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language Acquisition</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Linguistic Input</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Miniature artificial languages</topic><topic>Multilingualism</topic><topic>Native Speakers</topic><topic>Natural Language Processing</topic><topic>Probability learning</topic><topic>Psycholinguistics</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Regularization</topic><topic>Second language learning</topic><topic>Young Children</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hudson Kam, Carla L.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Newport, Elissa L.</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Cognitive psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hudson Kam, Carla L.</au><au>Newport, Elissa L.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ845728</ericid><atitle>Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages</atitle><jtitle>Cognitive psychology</jtitle><addtitle>Cogn Psychol</addtitle><date>2009-08-01</date><risdate>2009</risdate><volume>59</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>30</spage><epage>66</epage><pages>30-66</pages><issn>0010-0285</issn><eissn>1095-5623</eissn><coden>CGPSBQ</coden><abstract>When natural language input contains grammatical forms that are used probabilistically and inconsistently, learners will sometimes reproduce the inconsistencies; but sometimes they will instead regularize the use of these forms, introducing consistency in the language that was not present in the input. In this paper we ask what produces such regularization. We conducted three artificial language experiments, varying the use of determiners in the types of inconsistency with which they are used, and also comparing adult and child learners. In Experiment 1 we presented adult learners with
scattered inconsistency – the use of multiple determiners varying in frequency in the same context – and found that adults will reproduce these inconsistencies at low levels of scatter, but at very high levels of scatter will regularize the determiner system, producing the most frequent determiner form almost all the time. In Experiment 2 we showed that this is not merely the result of frequency: when determiners are used with low frequencies but in consistent contexts, adults will learn all of the determiners veridically. In Experiment 3 we compared adult and child learners, finding that children will almost always regularize inconsistent forms, whereas adult learners will only regularize the most complex inconsistencies. Taken together, these results suggest that regularization processes in natural language learning, such as those seen in the acquisition of language from non-native speakers or in the formation of young languages, may depend crucially on the nature of language learning by young children.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>19324332</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.01.001</doi><tpages>37</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0010-0285 |
ispartof | Cognitive psychology, 2009-08, Vol.59 (1), p.30-66 |
issn | 0010-0285 1095-5623 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_2703698 |
source | MEDLINE; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier) |
subjects | Adult Adult Basic Education Adult Learning Adult Students Adults Age Differences Age Factors Artificial Languages Bilingualism. Multilingualism Biological and medical sciences Child, Preschool Children Children & youth Cognition & reasoning Cognitive psychology Comparative Analysis English (Second Language) Error Analysis (Language) Experimental psychology Experiments Female Form Classes (Languages) Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Grammar Humans Inconsistency Language Language Acquisition Language Development Learning Linguistic Input Male Miniature artificial languages Multilingualism Native Speakers Natural Language Processing Probability learning Psycholinguistics Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Regularization Second language learning Young Children |
title | Getting it right by getting it wrong: When learners change languages |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-04T03%3A27%3A27IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Getting%20it%20right%20by%20getting%20it%20wrong:%20When%20learners%20change%20languages&rft.jtitle=Cognitive%20psychology&rft.au=Hudson%20Kam,%20Carla%20L.&rft.date=2009-08-01&rft.volume=59&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=30&rft.epage=66&rft.pages=30-66&rft.issn=0010-0285&rft.eissn=1095-5623&rft.coden=CGPSBQ&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.01.001&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E1768388591%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=213631748&rft_id=info:pmid/19324332&rft_ericid=EJ845728&rft_els_id=S0010028509000048&rfr_iscdi=true |