Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity
The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals w...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences Biological sciences, 2004-04, Vol.271 (1540), p.695-699 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 699 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1540 |
container_start_page | 695 |
container_title | Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences |
container_volume | 271 |
creator | Griffiths, S. W. Brockmark, S. Höjesjö, J. Johnsson, J. I. |
description | The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1098/rspb.2003.2648 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1691656</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>4142741</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>4142741</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkkuv0zAQhSME4l4ubFkhlBUrUjyOnQdIIKh4qhKP8pDYjNzESV3SONhOS_n1OE1VqBCXlWXPd8bneBwEt4FMgOTZA2O7xYQSEk9owrIrwTmwFCKac3Y1OCd5QqOMcXoW3LB2RQjJecavB2fAKcmB0PPg0VR3qq3DrXLLsFQbVcoyFM7J1indPgzdUoai3IjWiVqGugorsVaNEka53c3gWiUaK28d1ovg04vnH6evotnbl6-nT2dRkSaxi0QJIsvSLI0lcJKVEpKMLnLICsmIjHnFi7SQsiBVTMgi5RWhJcScMRoTQasqvgjuj33tVnb9Ajuj1sLsUAuFdd-hP6p7tBIZ-PQefzzinl3LsvBRjGhOVKeVVi2x1huEJIeEJ77BvUMDo7_30jpcK1vIphGt1L3FJBkoIP8FIWeEMAoenIxgYbS1RlZHN0BwmCMOc8RhjjjM0Qvu_pnhN34YnAfsCBi982-vCyXdDle6N63f4of5u2eQ52RDU1DAGUGSeceDa8CfqtvfNwDoAVTW9hL32KmPv23Fl936zzB3RtXKOm2OWRgwmrLhcaKxrKyTP45lYb5hksYpx88Zw_n7r7P5m9kcv3geRn6p6uVWGYknbvymM3axD7aPlOTca55cqhkMF7odvv2JEKu-8V-nrOJfZcIRpQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>19400421</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Griffiths, S. W. ; Brockmark, S. ; Höjesjö, J. ; Johnsson, J. I.</creator><creatorcontrib>Griffiths, S. W. ; Brockmark, S. ; Höjesjö, J. ; Johnsson, J. I.</creatorcontrib><description>The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0962-8452</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1471-2954</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2648</identifier><identifier>PMID: 15209102</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: The Royal Society</publisher><subject>Aggression - physiology ; Analysis of Variance ; Animal aggression ; Animals ; Anti-Predator Benefit ; Attention - physiology ; Biological Sciences ; Biologiska vetenskaper ; Body Weights and Measures ; Divided Attention ; Familiarity ; Feeding Behavior - physiology ; Feeding Behavior - psychology ; fighting behavior ; Fish ; Foraging ; Freshwater fishes ; Group Association ; heart-rate responses ; individual recognition ; juvenile atlantic salmon ; Larvae ; Limited Attention ; Observation ; poecilia-reticulata ; Predation ; predation risk ; Predators ; rainbow-trout ; Recognition ; Recognition (Psychology) - physiology ; Salmo trutta ; Salmon ; shoal ; Shoals ; Social Behavior ; Sweden ; Trout ; Trout - physiology</subject><ispartof>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences, 2004-04, Vol.271 (1540), p.695-699</ispartof><rights>Copyright 2004 The Royal Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/4142741$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/4142741$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,803,885,27924,27925,53791,53793,58017,58250</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15209102$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://gup.ub.gu.se/publication/41147$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Griffiths, S. W.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brockmark, S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Höjesjö, J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Johnsson, J. I.</creatorcontrib><title>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</title><title>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences</title><addtitle>Proc Biol Sci</addtitle><description>The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.</description><subject>Aggression - physiology</subject><subject>Analysis of Variance</subject><subject>Animal aggression</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Anti-Predator Benefit</subject><subject>Attention - physiology</subject><subject>Biological Sciences</subject><subject>Biologiska vetenskaper</subject><subject>Body Weights and Measures</subject><subject>Divided Attention</subject><subject>Familiarity</subject><subject>Feeding Behavior - physiology</subject><subject>Feeding Behavior - psychology</subject><subject>fighting behavior</subject><subject>Fish</subject><subject>Foraging</subject><subject>Freshwater fishes</subject><subject>Group Association</subject><subject>heart-rate responses</subject><subject>individual recognition</subject><subject>juvenile atlantic salmon</subject><subject>Larvae</subject><subject>Limited Attention</subject><subject>Observation</subject><subject>poecilia-reticulata</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>predation risk</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>rainbow-trout</subject><subject>Recognition</subject><subject>Recognition (Psychology) - physiology</subject><subject>Salmo trutta</subject><subject>Salmon</subject><subject>shoal</subject><subject>Shoals</subject><subject>Social Behavior</subject><subject>Sweden</subject><subject>Trout</subject><subject>Trout - physiology</subject><issn>0962-8452</issn><issn>1471-2954</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2004</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkkuv0zAQhSME4l4ubFkhlBUrUjyOnQdIIKh4qhKP8pDYjNzESV3SONhOS_n1OE1VqBCXlWXPd8bneBwEt4FMgOTZA2O7xYQSEk9owrIrwTmwFCKac3Y1OCd5QqOMcXoW3LB2RQjJecavB2fAKcmB0PPg0VR3qq3DrXLLsFQbVcoyFM7J1indPgzdUoai3IjWiVqGugorsVaNEka53c3gWiUaK28d1ovg04vnH6evotnbl6-nT2dRkSaxi0QJIsvSLI0lcJKVEpKMLnLICsmIjHnFi7SQsiBVTMgi5RWhJcScMRoTQasqvgjuj33tVnb9Ajuj1sLsUAuFdd-hP6p7tBIZ-PQefzzinl3LsvBRjGhOVKeVVi2x1huEJIeEJ77BvUMDo7_30jpcK1vIphGt1L3FJBkoIP8FIWeEMAoenIxgYbS1RlZHN0BwmCMOc8RhjjjM0Qvu_pnhN34YnAfsCBi982-vCyXdDle6N63f4of5u2eQ52RDU1DAGUGSeceDa8CfqtvfNwDoAVTW9hL32KmPv23Fl936zzB3RtXKOm2OWRgwmrLhcaKxrKyTP45lYb5hksYpx88Zw_n7r7P5m9kcv3geRn6p6uVWGYknbvymM3axD7aPlOTca55cqhkMF7odvv2JEKu-8V-nrOJfZcIRpQ</recordid><startdate>20040407</startdate><enddate>20040407</enddate><creator>Griffiths, S. W.</creator><creator>Brockmark, S.</creator><creator>Höjesjö, J.</creator><creator>Johnsson, J. I.</creator><general>The Royal Society</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>F1W</scope><scope>H95</scope><scope>L.G</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>ADTPV</scope><scope>AOWAS</scope><scope>F1U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20040407</creationdate><title>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</title><author>Griffiths, S. W. ; Brockmark, S. ; Höjesjö, J. ; Johnsson, J. I.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2004</creationdate><topic>Aggression - physiology</topic><topic>Analysis of Variance</topic><topic>Animal aggression</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Anti-Predator Benefit</topic><topic>Attention - physiology</topic><topic>Biological Sciences</topic><topic>Biologiska vetenskaper</topic><topic>Body Weights and Measures</topic><topic>Divided Attention</topic><topic>Familiarity</topic><topic>Feeding Behavior - physiology</topic><topic>Feeding Behavior - psychology</topic><topic>fighting behavior</topic><topic>Fish</topic><topic>Foraging</topic><topic>Freshwater fishes</topic><topic>Group Association</topic><topic>heart-rate responses</topic><topic>individual recognition</topic><topic>juvenile atlantic salmon</topic><topic>Larvae</topic><topic>Limited Attention</topic><topic>Observation</topic><topic>poecilia-reticulata</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>predation risk</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>rainbow-trout</topic><topic>Recognition</topic><topic>Recognition (Psychology) - physiology</topic><topic>Salmo trutta</topic><topic>Salmon</topic><topic>shoal</topic><topic>Shoals</topic><topic>Social Behavior</topic><topic>Sweden</topic><topic>Trout</topic><topic>Trout - physiology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Griffiths, S. W.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brockmark, S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Höjesjö, J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Johnsson, J. I.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>ASFA: Aquatic Sciences and Fisheries Abstracts</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) 1: Biological Sciences & Living Resources</collection><collection>Aquatic Science & Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) Professional</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>SwePub</collection><collection>SwePub Articles</collection><collection>SWEPUB Göteborgs universitet</collection><jtitle>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Griffiths, S. W.</au><au>Brockmark, S.</au><au>Höjesjö, J.</au><au>Johnsson, J. I.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</atitle><jtitle>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences</jtitle><addtitle>Proc Biol Sci</addtitle><date>2004-04-07</date><risdate>2004</risdate><volume>271</volume><issue>1540</issue><spage>695</spage><epage>699</epage><pages>695-699</pages><issn>0962-8452</issn><eissn>1471-2954</eissn><abstract>The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>The Royal Society</pub><pmid>15209102</pmid><doi>10.1098/rspb.2003.2648</doi><tpages>5</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0962-8452 |
ispartof | Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences, 2004-04, Vol.271 (1540), p.695-699 |
issn | 0962-8452 1471-2954 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1691656 |
source | MEDLINE; JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing; PubMed Central |
subjects | Aggression - physiology Analysis of Variance Animal aggression Animals Anti-Predator Benefit Attention - physiology Biological Sciences Biologiska vetenskaper Body Weights and Measures Divided Attention Familiarity Feeding Behavior - physiology Feeding Behavior - psychology fighting behavior Fish Foraging Freshwater fishes Group Association heart-rate responses individual recognition juvenile atlantic salmon Larvae Limited Attention Observation poecilia-reticulata Predation predation risk Predators rainbow-trout Recognition Recognition (Psychology) - physiology Salmo trutta Salmon shoal Shoals Social Behavior Sweden Trout Trout - physiology |
title | Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-21T07%3A40%3A07IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Coping%20with%20divided%20attention:%20the%20advantage%20of%20familiarity&rft.jtitle=Proceedings%20of%20the%20Royal%20Society.%20B,%20Biological%20sciences&rft.au=Griffiths,%20S.%20W.&rft.date=2004-04-07&rft.volume=271&rft.issue=1540&rft.spage=695&rft.epage=699&rft.pages=695-699&rft.issn=0962-8452&rft.eissn=1471-2954&rft_id=info:doi/10.1098/rspb.2003.2648&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_pubme%3E4142741%3C/jstor_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=19400421&rft_id=info:pmid/15209102&rft_jstor_id=4142741&rfr_iscdi=true |