Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity

The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals w...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences Biological sciences, 2004-04, Vol.271 (1540), p.695-699
Hauptverfasser: Griffiths, S. W., Brockmark, S., Höjesjö, J., Johnsson, J. I.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 699
container_issue 1540
container_start_page 695
container_title Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences
container_volume 271
creator Griffiths, S. W.
Brockmark, S.
Höjesjö, J.
Johnsson, J. I.
description The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.
doi_str_mv 10.1098/rspb.2003.2648
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstor_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1691656</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>4142741</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>4142741</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkkuv0zAQhSME4l4ubFkhlBUrUjyOnQdIIKh4qhKP8pDYjNzESV3SONhOS_n1OE1VqBCXlWXPd8bneBwEt4FMgOTZA2O7xYQSEk9owrIrwTmwFCKac3Y1OCd5QqOMcXoW3LB2RQjJecavB2fAKcmB0PPg0VR3qq3DrXLLsFQbVcoyFM7J1indPgzdUoai3IjWiVqGugorsVaNEka53c3gWiUaK28d1ovg04vnH6evotnbl6-nT2dRkSaxi0QJIsvSLI0lcJKVEpKMLnLICsmIjHnFi7SQsiBVTMgi5RWhJcScMRoTQasqvgjuj33tVnb9Ajuj1sLsUAuFdd-hP6p7tBIZ-PQefzzinl3LsvBRjGhOVKeVVi2x1huEJIeEJ77BvUMDo7_30jpcK1vIphGt1L3FJBkoIP8FIWeEMAoenIxgYbS1RlZHN0BwmCMOc8RhjjjM0Qvu_pnhN34YnAfsCBi982-vCyXdDle6N63f4of5u2eQ52RDU1DAGUGSeceDa8CfqtvfNwDoAVTW9hL32KmPv23Fl936zzB3RtXKOm2OWRgwmrLhcaKxrKyTP45lYb5hksYpx88Zw_n7r7P5m9kcv3geRn6p6uVWGYknbvymM3axD7aPlOTca55cqhkMF7odvv2JEKu-8V-nrOJfZcIRpQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>19400421</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><source>PubMed Central</source><creator>Griffiths, S. W. ; Brockmark, S. ; Höjesjö, J. ; Johnsson, J. I.</creator><creatorcontrib>Griffiths, S. W. ; Brockmark, S. ; Höjesjö, J. ; Johnsson, J. I.</creatorcontrib><description>The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0962-8452</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1471-2954</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2648</identifier><identifier>PMID: 15209102</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: The Royal Society</publisher><subject>Aggression - physiology ; Analysis of Variance ; Animal aggression ; Animals ; Anti-Predator Benefit ; Attention - physiology ; Biological Sciences ; Biologiska vetenskaper ; Body Weights and Measures ; Divided Attention ; Familiarity ; Feeding Behavior - physiology ; Feeding Behavior - psychology ; fighting behavior ; Fish ; Foraging ; Freshwater fishes ; Group Association ; heart-rate responses ; individual recognition ; juvenile atlantic salmon ; Larvae ; Limited Attention ; Observation ; poecilia-reticulata ; Predation ; predation risk ; Predators ; rainbow-trout ; Recognition ; Recognition (Psychology) - physiology ; Salmo trutta ; Salmon ; shoal ; Shoals ; Social Behavior ; Sweden ; Trout ; Trout - physiology</subject><ispartof>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences, 2004-04, Vol.271 (1540), p.695-699</ispartof><rights>Copyright 2004 The Royal Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/4142741$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/4142741$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,803,885,27924,27925,53791,53793,58017,58250</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15209102$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://gup.ub.gu.se/publication/41147$$DView record from Swedish Publication Index$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Griffiths, S. W.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brockmark, S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Höjesjö, J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Johnsson, J. I.</creatorcontrib><title>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</title><title>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences</title><addtitle>Proc Biol Sci</addtitle><description>The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.</description><subject>Aggression - physiology</subject><subject>Analysis of Variance</subject><subject>Animal aggression</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Anti-Predator Benefit</subject><subject>Attention - physiology</subject><subject>Biological Sciences</subject><subject>Biologiska vetenskaper</subject><subject>Body Weights and Measures</subject><subject>Divided Attention</subject><subject>Familiarity</subject><subject>Feeding Behavior - physiology</subject><subject>Feeding Behavior - psychology</subject><subject>fighting behavior</subject><subject>Fish</subject><subject>Foraging</subject><subject>Freshwater fishes</subject><subject>Group Association</subject><subject>heart-rate responses</subject><subject>individual recognition</subject><subject>juvenile atlantic salmon</subject><subject>Larvae</subject><subject>Limited Attention</subject><subject>Observation</subject><subject>poecilia-reticulata</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>predation risk</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>rainbow-trout</subject><subject>Recognition</subject><subject>Recognition (Psychology) - physiology</subject><subject>Salmo trutta</subject><subject>Salmon</subject><subject>shoal</subject><subject>Shoals</subject><subject>Social Behavior</subject><subject>Sweden</subject><subject>Trout</subject><subject>Trout - physiology</subject><issn>0962-8452</issn><issn>1471-2954</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2004</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkkuv0zAQhSME4l4ubFkhlBUrUjyOnQdIIKh4qhKP8pDYjNzESV3SONhOS_n1OE1VqBCXlWXPd8bneBwEt4FMgOTZA2O7xYQSEk9owrIrwTmwFCKac3Y1OCd5QqOMcXoW3LB2RQjJecavB2fAKcmB0PPg0VR3qq3DrXLLsFQbVcoyFM7J1indPgzdUoai3IjWiVqGugorsVaNEka53c3gWiUaK28d1ovg04vnH6evotnbl6-nT2dRkSaxi0QJIsvSLI0lcJKVEpKMLnLICsmIjHnFi7SQsiBVTMgi5RWhJcScMRoTQasqvgjuj33tVnb9Ajuj1sLsUAuFdd-hP6p7tBIZ-PQefzzinl3LsvBRjGhOVKeVVi2x1huEJIeEJ77BvUMDo7_30jpcK1vIphGt1L3FJBkoIP8FIWeEMAoenIxgYbS1RlZHN0BwmCMOc8RhjjjM0Qvu_pnhN34YnAfsCBi982-vCyXdDle6N63f4of5u2eQ52RDU1DAGUGSeceDa8CfqtvfNwDoAVTW9hL32KmPv23Fl936zzB3RtXKOm2OWRgwmrLhcaKxrKyTP45lYb5hksYpx88Zw_n7r7P5m9kcv3geRn6p6uVWGYknbvymM3axD7aPlOTca55cqhkMF7odvv2JEKu-8V-nrOJfZcIRpQ</recordid><startdate>20040407</startdate><enddate>20040407</enddate><creator>Griffiths, S. W.</creator><creator>Brockmark, S.</creator><creator>Höjesjö, J.</creator><creator>Johnsson, J. I.</creator><general>The Royal Society</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>F1W</scope><scope>H95</scope><scope>L.G</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><scope>ADTPV</scope><scope>AOWAS</scope><scope>F1U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20040407</creationdate><title>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</title><author>Griffiths, S. W. ; Brockmark, S. ; Höjesjö, J. ; Johnsson, J. I.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c763t-ad1a887873e1508de1682b918ce40e35f5c7ceec0f300b75f02d13544230a2ff3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2004</creationdate><topic>Aggression - physiology</topic><topic>Analysis of Variance</topic><topic>Animal aggression</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Anti-Predator Benefit</topic><topic>Attention - physiology</topic><topic>Biological Sciences</topic><topic>Biologiska vetenskaper</topic><topic>Body Weights and Measures</topic><topic>Divided Attention</topic><topic>Familiarity</topic><topic>Feeding Behavior - physiology</topic><topic>Feeding Behavior - psychology</topic><topic>fighting behavior</topic><topic>Fish</topic><topic>Foraging</topic><topic>Freshwater fishes</topic><topic>Group Association</topic><topic>heart-rate responses</topic><topic>individual recognition</topic><topic>juvenile atlantic salmon</topic><topic>Larvae</topic><topic>Limited Attention</topic><topic>Observation</topic><topic>poecilia-reticulata</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>predation risk</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>rainbow-trout</topic><topic>Recognition</topic><topic>Recognition (Psychology) - physiology</topic><topic>Salmo trutta</topic><topic>Salmon</topic><topic>shoal</topic><topic>Shoals</topic><topic>Social Behavior</topic><topic>Sweden</topic><topic>Trout</topic><topic>Trout - physiology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Griffiths, S. W.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brockmark, S.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Höjesjö, J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Johnsson, J. I.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>ASFA: Aquatic Sciences and Fisheries Abstracts</collection><collection>Aquatic Science &amp; Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) 1: Biological Sciences &amp; Living Resources</collection><collection>Aquatic Science &amp; Fisheries Abstracts (ASFA) Professional</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><collection>SwePub</collection><collection>SwePub Articles</collection><collection>SWEPUB Göteborgs universitet</collection><jtitle>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Griffiths, S. W.</au><au>Brockmark, S.</au><au>Höjesjö, J.</au><au>Johnsson, J. I.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity</atitle><jtitle>Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences</jtitle><addtitle>Proc Biol Sci</addtitle><date>2004-04-07</date><risdate>2004</risdate><volume>271</volume><issue>1540</issue><spage>695</spage><epage>699</epage><pages>695-699</pages><issn>0962-8452</issn><eissn>1471-2954</eissn><abstract>The ability of an animal to perform a task successfully is limited by the amount of attention being simultaneously focused on other activities. One way in which individuals might reduce the cost of divided attention is by preferentially focusing on the most beneficial tasks. In territorial animals where aggression is lower among familiar individuals, the decision to associate preferentially with familiar conspecifics may therefore confer advantages by allowing attention to be switched from aggression to predator vigilance and feeding. Wild juvenile brown trout were used to test the prediction that familiar fishes respond more quickly than unfamiliar fishes to a simulated predator attack. Our results confirm this prediction by demonstrating that familiar trout respond 14% faster than unfamiliar individuals to a predator attack. The results also show that familiar fishes consume a greater number of food items, foraging at more than twice the rate of unfamiliar conspecifics. To the best of our knowledge, these results provide the first evidence that familiarity-biased association confers advantages through the immediate fitness benefits afforded by faster predator-evasion responses and the long-term benefits provided by increased feeding opportunities.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>The Royal Society</pub><pmid>15209102</pmid><doi>10.1098/rspb.2003.2648</doi><tpages>5</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0962-8452
ispartof Proceedings of the Royal Society. B, Biological sciences, 2004-04, Vol.271 (1540), p.695-699
issn 0962-8452
1471-2954
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1691656
source MEDLINE; JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing; PubMed Central
subjects Aggression - physiology
Analysis of Variance
Animal aggression
Animals
Anti-Predator Benefit
Attention - physiology
Biological Sciences
Biologiska vetenskaper
Body Weights and Measures
Divided Attention
Familiarity
Feeding Behavior - physiology
Feeding Behavior - psychology
fighting behavior
Fish
Foraging
Freshwater fishes
Group Association
heart-rate responses
individual recognition
juvenile atlantic salmon
Larvae
Limited Attention
Observation
poecilia-reticulata
Predation
predation risk
Predators
rainbow-trout
Recognition
Recognition (Psychology) - physiology
Salmo trutta
Salmon
shoal
Shoals
Social Behavior
Sweden
Trout
Trout - physiology
title Coping with divided attention: the advantage of familiarity
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-21T07%3A40%3A07IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Coping%20with%20divided%20attention:%20the%20advantage%20of%20familiarity&rft.jtitle=Proceedings%20of%20the%20Royal%20Society.%20B,%20Biological%20sciences&rft.au=Griffiths,%20S.%20W.&rft.date=2004-04-07&rft.volume=271&rft.issue=1540&rft.spage=695&rft.epage=699&rft.pages=695-699&rft.issn=0962-8452&rft.eissn=1471-2954&rft_id=info:doi/10.1098/rspb.2003.2648&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_pubme%3E4142741%3C/jstor_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=19400421&rft_id=info:pmid/15209102&rft_jstor_id=4142741&rfr_iscdi=true