SELF-REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS: AN ARTIFACT OF SOCIAL STANDARD SETTING?

Two studies were conducted to identify mechanisms responsible for observed “self‐reinforcement” effects. In Experiment 1, using a studying task, self‐reinforcement procedures did not work when they were private (i.e., when others are not aware of the goals or contingencies), but did work when they w...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of applied behavior analysis 1985, Vol.18 (3), p.201-214
Hauptverfasser: Hayes, Steven C., Rosenfarb, Irwin, Wulfert, Edelgard, Munt, Edwin D., Korn, Zamir, Zettle, Robert D.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 214
container_issue 3
container_start_page 201
container_title Journal of applied behavior analysis
container_volume 18
creator Hayes, Steven C.
Rosenfarb, Irwin
Wulfert, Edelgard
Munt, Edwin D.
Korn, Zamir
Zettle, Robert D.
description Two studies were conducted to identify mechanisms responsible for observed “self‐reinforcement” effects. In Experiment 1, using a studying task, self‐reinforcement procedures did not work when they were private (i.e., when others are not aware of the goals or contingencies), but did work when they were public. Self‐delivery of consequences added nothing to the effectiveness of the procedure. The data suggested that public goal setting was the critical element in the procedure's effectiveness. In Experiment 2, an applied extension, goal setting alone was effective in modifying over a long time period studying behaviors of people with significant studying difficulties, but only when the goals were known to others. Overall, the two experiments make more plausible the view that self‐reinforcement procedures work by setting a socially available standard against which performance can be evaluated. The procedure itself functions as a discriminative stimulus for stringent or lenient social contingencies. The application of this mechanism to other problems of applied significance is briefly discussed.
doi_str_mv 10.1901/jaba.1985.18-201
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1308011</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1311947494</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c6033-95c1cf209a61ee9af2ba222930d437cc1dfce1a81ea0c3935d12ae83be8d93163</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkc9v0zAYhi0EYmVw54QigbRThj87TmwOoCxLSkZJpSSIo-U6DqRLkxG3wP57XLUqPy6cbMnP-9qfH4SeA74EgeH1Wq2U23F2CdwnGB6gGQjKfRph-hDNMCbgc87YGXpi7RpjIDhkj9EZhJFgIeczlFTpIvPLNC-yZZmkH9Oi9tIsS5O6euPFhReXdZ7FSe0tM69aJnm88Ko6Lq7j8tqr0rrOi_m7p-hRq3prnh3Xc_QpS-vkvb9YzvMkXvg6xJT6gmnQLcFChWCMUC1ZKUKIoLgJaKQ1NK02oDgYhTUVlDVAlOF0ZXgjKIT0HL099N7tVhvTaDNsJ9XLu6nbqOlejqqTf58M3Vf5ZfwugWKOAVzBxbFgGr_tjN3KTWe16Xs1mHFnZURpEHEGzJEv_yHX424a3HSuDEAEUSACR-EDpafR2sm0p7cAlntBci9I7gVJ4NIJcpEXf87wO3A04oBXR0BZrfp2UoPu7InjYcSB7_8iPGA_ut7c__deeRNfxYRT6oL-IdjZrfl5CqrpVoYRjZj8XMxlXVbJjTMlP9BfOUKyQQ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1311947494</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>SELF-REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS: AN ARTIFACT OF SOCIAL STANDARD SETTING?</title><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Hayes, Steven C. ; Rosenfarb, Irwin ; Wulfert, Edelgard ; Munt, Edwin D. ; Korn, Zamir ; Zettle, Robert D.</creator><creatorcontrib>Hayes, Steven C. ; Rosenfarb, Irwin ; Wulfert, Edelgard ; Munt, Edwin D. ; Korn, Zamir ; Zettle, Robert D.</creatorcontrib><description>Two studies were conducted to identify mechanisms responsible for observed “self‐reinforcement” effects. In Experiment 1, using a studying task, self‐reinforcement procedures did not work when they were private (i.e., when others are not aware of the goals or contingencies), but did work when they were public. Self‐delivery of consequences added nothing to the effectiveness of the procedure. The data suggested that public goal setting was the critical element in the procedure's effectiveness. In Experiment 2, an applied extension, goal setting alone was effective in modifying over a long time period studying behaviors of people with significant studying difficulties, but only when the goals were known to others. Overall, the two experiments make more plausible the view that self‐reinforcement procedures work by setting a socially available standard against which performance can be evaluated. The procedure itself functions as a discriminative stimulus for stringent or lenient social contingencies. The application of this mechanism to other problems of applied significance is briefly discussed.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0021-8855</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1938-3703</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1901/jaba.1985.18-201</identifier><identifier>PMID: 16795688</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JOABAW</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing Ltd</publisher><subject>Biological and medical sciences ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; goal setting ; Personality traits ; Personality. Affectivity ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; public knowledge of treatment ; self-control ; self-reinforcement ; social standard setting</subject><ispartof>Journal of applied behavior analysis, 1985, Vol.18 (3), p.201-214</ispartof><rights>1985 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</rights><rights>1986 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c6033-95c1cf209a61ee9af2ba222930d437cc1dfce1a81ea0c3935d12ae83be8d93163</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c6033-95c1cf209a61ee9af2ba222930d437cc1dfce1a81ea0c3935d12ae83be8d93163</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1308011/pdf/$$EPDF$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1308011/$$EHTML$$P50$$Gpubmedcentral$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,881,27847,27902,27903,53768,53770</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=8678186$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16795688$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hayes, Steven C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rosenfarb, Irwin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wulfert, Edelgard</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Munt, Edwin D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Korn, Zamir</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Zettle, Robert D.</creatorcontrib><title>SELF-REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS: AN ARTIFACT OF SOCIAL STANDARD SETTING?</title><title>Journal of applied behavior analysis</title><addtitle>J Appl Behav Anal</addtitle><description>Two studies were conducted to identify mechanisms responsible for observed “self‐reinforcement” effects. In Experiment 1, using a studying task, self‐reinforcement procedures did not work when they were private (i.e., when others are not aware of the goals or contingencies), but did work when they were public. Self‐delivery of consequences added nothing to the effectiveness of the procedure. The data suggested that public goal setting was the critical element in the procedure's effectiveness. In Experiment 2, an applied extension, goal setting alone was effective in modifying over a long time period studying behaviors of people with significant studying difficulties, but only when the goals were known to others. Overall, the two experiments make more plausible the view that self‐reinforcement procedures work by setting a socially available standard against which performance can be evaluated. The procedure itself functions as a discriminative stimulus for stringent or lenient social contingencies. The application of this mechanism to other problems of applied significance is briefly discussed.</description><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>goal setting</subject><subject>Personality traits</subject><subject>Personality. Affectivity</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>public knowledge of treatment</subject><subject>self-control</subject><subject>self-reinforcement</subject><subject>social standard setting</subject><issn>0021-8855</issn><issn>1938-3703</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1985</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkc9v0zAYhi0EYmVw54QigbRThj87TmwOoCxLSkZJpSSIo-U6DqRLkxG3wP57XLUqPy6cbMnP-9qfH4SeA74EgeH1Wq2U23F2CdwnGB6gGQjKfRph-hDNMCbgc87YGXpi7RpjIDhkj9EZhJFgIeczlFTpIvPLNC-yZZmkH9Oi9tIsS5O6euPFhReXdZ7FSe0tM69aJnm88Ko6Lq7j8tqr0rrOi_m7p-hRq3prnh3Xc_QpS-vkvb9YzvMkXvg6xJT6gmnQLcFChWCMUC1ZKUKIoLgJaKQ1NK02oDgYhTUVlDVAlOF0ZXgjKIT0HL099N7tVhvTaDNsJ9XLu6nbqOlejqqTf58M3Vf5ZfwugWKOAVzBxbFgGr_tjN3KTWe16Xs1mHFnZURpEHEGzJEv_yHX424a3HSuDEAEUSACR-EDpafR2sm0p7cAlntBci9I7gVJ4NIJcpEXf87wO3A04oBXR0BZrfp2UoPu7InjYcSB7_8iPGA_ut7c__deeRNfxYRT6oL-IdjZrfl5CqrpVoYRjZj8XMxlXVbJjTMlP9BfOUKyQQ</recordid><startdate>1985</startdate><enddate>1985</enddate><creator>Hayes, Steven C.</creator><creator>Rosenfarb, Irwin</creator><creator>Wulfert, Edelgard</creator><creator>Munt, Edwin D.</creator><creator>Korn, Zamir</creator><creator>Zettle, Robert D.</creator><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><general>Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>JRZRW</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>SFNNT</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>1985</creationdate><title>SELF-REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS: AN ARTIFACT OF SOCIAL STANDARD SETTING?</title><author>Hayes, Steven C. ; Rosenfarb, Irwin ; Wulfert, Edelgard ; Munt, Edwin D. ; Korn, Zamir ; Zettle, Robert D.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c6033-95c1cf209a61ee9af2ba222930d437cc1dfce1a81ea0c3935d12ae83be8d93163</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1985</creationdate><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>goal setting</topic><topic>Personality traits</topic><topic>Personality. Affectivity</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>public knowledge of treatment</topic><topic>self-control</topic><topic>self-reinforcement</topic><topic>social standard setting</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hayes, Steven C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rosenfarb, Irwin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wulfert, Edelgard</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Munt, Edwin D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Korn, Zamir</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Zettle, Robert D.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 35</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 44</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Journal of applied behavior analysis</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hayes, Steven C.</au><au>Rosenfarb, Irwin</au><au>Wulfert, Edelgard</au><au>Munt, Edwin D.</au><au>Korn, Zamir</au><au>Zettle, Robert D.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>SELF-REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS: AN ARTIFACT OF SOCIAL STANDARD SETTING?</atitle><jtitle>Journal of applied behavior analysis</jtitle><addtitle>J Appl Behav Anal</addtitle><date>1985</date><risdate>1985</risdate><volume>18</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>201</spage><epage>214</epage><pages>201-214</pages><issn>0021-8855</issn><eissn>1938-3703</eissn><coden>JOABAW</coden><abstract>Two studies were conducted to identify mechanisms responsible for observed “self‐reinforcement” effects. In Experiment 1, using a studying task, self‐reinforcement procedures did not work when they were private (i.e., when others are not aware of the goals or contingencies), but did work when they were public. Self‐delivery of consequences added nothing to the effectiveness of the procedure. The data suggested that public goal setting was the critical element in the procedure's effectiveness. In Experiment 2, an applied extension, goal setting alone was effective in modifying over a long time period studying behaviors of people with significant studying difficulties, but only when the goals were known to others. Overall, the two experiments make more plausible the view that self‐reinforcement procedures work by setting a socially available standard against which performance can be evaluated. The procedure itself functions as a discriminative stimulus for stringent or lenient social contingencies. The application of this mechanism to other problems of applied significance is briefly discussed.</abstract><cop>Oxford, UK</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</pub><pmid>16795688</pmid><doi>10.1901/jaba.1985.18-201</doi><tpages>14</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0021-8855
ispartof Journal of applied behavior analysis, 1985, Vol.18 (3), p.201-214
issn 0021-8855
1938-3703
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1308011
source EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals; PubMed Central; Periodicals Index Online
subjects Biological and medical sciences
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
goal setting
Personality traits
Personality. Affectivity
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
public knowledge of treatment
self-control
self-reinforcement
social standard setting
title SELF-REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS: AN ARTIFACT OF SOCIAL STANDARD SETTING?
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-27T09%3A56%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=SELF-REINFORCEMENT%20EFFECTS:%20AN%20ARTIFACT%20OF%20SOCIAL%20STANDARD%20SETTING?&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20applied%20behavior%20analysis&rft.au=Hayes,%20Steven%20C.&rft.date=1985&rft.volume=18&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=201&rft.epage=214&rft.pages=201-214&rft.issn=0021-8855&rft.eissn=1938-3703&rft.coden=JOABAW&rft_id=info:doi/10.1901/jaba.1985.18-201&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E1311947494%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1311947494&rft_id=info:pmid/16795688&rfr_iscdi=true