Mountain Gorilla Tug-of-War: Silverbacks Have Limited Control over Reproduction in Multimale Groups
To determine who fathers the offspring in wild mountain gorilla groups containing more than one adult male silverback, we genotyped nearly one-fourth (n = 92) of the mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) living in the Virunga Volcanoes region of Africa. Paternity analysis of 48 offspring bor...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS 2005-06, Vol.102 (26), p.9418-9423 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 9423 |
---|---|
container_issue | 26 |
container_start_page | 9418 |
container_title | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS |
container_volume | 102 |
creator | Bradley, Brenda J. Robbins, Martha M. Williamson, Elizabeth A. Steklis, H. Dieter Steklis, Netzin Gerald Eckhardt, Nadin Boesch, Christophe Vigilant, Linda Frans B. M. de Waal |
description | To determine who fathers the offspring in wild mountain gorilla groups containing more than one adult male silverback, we genotyped nearly one-fourth (n = 92) of the mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) living in the Virunga Volcanoes region of Africa. Paternity analysis of 48 offspring born into four groups between 1985 and 1999 revealed that, although all infants were sired by within-group males, the socially dominant silverback did not always monopolize reproduction within his group. Instead, the second-ranking male sired an average of 15% of group offspring. This result, in combination with previous findings that second-ranking males fare best by not leaving the group but by staying and waiting to assume dominance even if no reproduction is possible while waiting, is not consistent with expectations from a reproductive skew model in which the silverback concedes controllable reproduction to the second-ranking male. Instead, the data suggest a "tug-of-war" scenario in which neither the dominant nor the second-ranking male has full control over his relative reproductive share. The two top-ranked males were typically unrelated and this, in combination with the mixed paternity of group offspring, means that multimale gorilla groups do not approximate family groups. Instead, as long-term assemblages of related and unrelated individuals, gorilla groups are similar to chimpanzee groups and so offer interesting possibilities for kinbiased interactions among individuals. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1073/pnas.0502019102 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1166604</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>3375910</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>3375910</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-b311934fdd6bf36b1c6c1e2de89f938198c520bcee6d34c93ab194afe508bd143</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0c2LEzEYBvAgittdPXsRHfYgXmb3zSSTSTwIUrQrdBF0xWPIZDJrajqp-Sj635vSslUPesohvzy8eR-EnmC4wNCRy82k4gW00AAWGJp7aIZB4JpRAffRDKDpak4beoJOY1wBgGg5PEQnuBWFcDpD-trnKSk7VQsfrHOqusm3tR_rLyq8qj5ZtzWhV_pbrK7U1lRLu7bJDNXcTyl4V_lyXX00m-CHrJP1U1WSrrNLdq2cqRbB5018hB6MykXz-HCeoc_v3t7Mr-rlh8X7-ZtlrVtBUt0TjAWh4zCwfiSsx5ppbJrBcDEKwrHgum2g18awgVAtiOqxoGo0LfB-wJScodf73E3u12bQpsyonNyEMkz4Kb2y8s-byX6Vt34rMWaMwS7gxSEg-O_ZxCTXNmpTtjIZn6NkneCcQvtfiDtGOui6As__giufw1S2IEtlpGO0ZQVd7pEOPsZgxruRMchdzXJXszzWXF48-_2nR3_otYDnB7B7eYxrZMOkoJgX8fLfQo7ZuWR-pEKf7ukqJh_uLCFdW6YhvwDmucaC</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>201376456</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Mountain Gorilla Tug-of-War: Silverbacks Have Limited Control over Reproduction in Multimale Groups</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><source>MEDLINE</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Bradley, Brenda J. ; Robbins, Martha M. ; Williamson, Elizabeth A. ; Steklis, H. Dieter ; Steklis, Netzin Gerald ; Eckhardt, Nadin ; Boesch, Christophe ; Vigilant, Linda ; Frans B. M. de Waal</creator><creatorcontrib>Bradley, Brenda J. ; Robbins, Martha M. ; Williamson, Elizabeth A. ; Steklis, H. Dieter ; Steklis, Netzin Gerald ; Eckhardt, Nadin ; Boesch, Christophe ; Vigilant, Linda ; Frans B. M. de Waal</creatorcontrib><description>To determine who fathers the offspring in wild mountain gorilla groups containing more than one adult male silverback, we genotyped nearly one-fourth (n = 92) of the mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) living in the Virunga Volcanoes region of Africa. Paternity analysis of 48 offspring born into four groups between 1985 and 1999 revealed that, although all infants were sired by within-group males, the socially dominant silverback did not always monopolize reproduction within his group. Instead, the second-ranking male sired an average of 15% of group offspring. This result, in combination with previous findings that second-ranking males fare best by not leaving the group but by staying and waiting to assume dominance even if no reproduction is possible while waiting, is not consistent with expectations from a reproductive skew model in which the silverback concedes controllable reproduction to the second-ranking male. Instead, the data suggest a "tug-of-war" scenario in which neither the dominant nor the second-ranking male has full control over his relative reproductive share. The two top-ranked males were typically unrelated and this, in combination with the mixed paternity of group offspring, means that multimale gorilla groups do not approximate family groups. Instead, as long-term assemblages of related and unrelated individuals, gorilla groups are similar to chimpanzee groups and so offer interesting possibilities for kinbiased interactions among individuals.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0027-8424</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1091-6490</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0502019102</identifier><identifier>PMID: 15964984</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: National Academy of Sciences</publisher><subject>Alleles ; Animal reproduction ; Animals ; Behavior, Animal ; Ecological modeling ; Female ; Genotype ; Genotypes ; Gorilla gorilla ; Gorillas ; Hierarchy, Social ; Kinship ; Male ; Male animals ; Males ; Mating behavior ; Microsatellite Repeats ; Modeling ; Models, Biological ; Mountains ; Paternity ; Population Dynamics ; Reproduction ; Reproductive success ; Sexual Behavior, Animal ; Siblings ; Social Dominance ; Social Sciences ; Species Specificity ; Time Factors</subject><ispartof>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 2005-06, Vol.102 (26), p.9418-9423</ispartof><rights>Copyright 1993/2005 The National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America</rights><rights>Copyright National Academy of Sciences Jun 28, 2005</rights><rights>Copyright © 2005, The National Academy of Sciences 2005</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-b311934fdd6bf36b1c6c1e2de89f938198c520bcee6d34c93ab194afe508bd143</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-b311934fdd6bf36b1c6c1e2de89f938198c520bcee6d34c93ab194afe508bd143</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Uhttp://www.pnas.org/content/102/26.cover.gif</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/3375910$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/3375910$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,723,776,780,799,881,27901,27902,53766,53768,57992,58225</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15964984$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Bradley, Brenda J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Robbins, Martha M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Williamson, Elizabeth A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steklis, H. Dieter</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steklis, Netzin Gerald</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Eckhardt, Nadin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Boesch, Christophe</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vigilant, Linda</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Frans B. M. de Waal</creatorcontrib><title>Mountain Gorilla Tug-of-War: Silverbacks Have Limited Control over Reproduction in Multimale Groups</title><title>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS</title><addtitle>Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A</addtitle><description>To determine who fathers the offspring in wild mountain gorilla groups containing more than one adult male silverback, we genotyped nearly one-fourth (n = 92) of the mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) living in the Virunga Volcanoes region of Africa. Paternity analysis of 48 offspring born into four groups between 1985 and 1999 revealed that, although all infants were sired by within-group males, the socially dominant silverback did not always monopolize reproduction within his group. Instead, the second-ranking male sired an average of 15% of group offspring. This result, in combination with previous findings that second-ranking males fare best by not leaving the group but by staying and waiting to assume dominance even if no reproduction is possible while waiting, is not consistent with expectations from a reproductive skew model in which the silverback concedes controllable reproduction to the second-ranking male. Instead, the data suggest a "tug-of-war" scenario in which neither the dominant nor the second-ranking male has full control over his relative reproductive share. The two top-ranked males were typically unrelated and this, in combination with the mixed paternity of group offspring, means that multimale gorilla groups do not approximate family groups. Instead, as long-term assemblages of related and unrelated individuals, gorilla groups are similar to chimpanzee groups and so offer interesting possibilities for kinbiased interactions among individuals.</description><subject>Alleles</subject><subject>Animal reproduction</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Behavior, Animal</subject><subject>Ecological modeling</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Genotype</subject><subject>Genotypes</subject><subject>Gorilla gorilla</subject><subject>Gorillas</subject><subject>Hierarchy, Social</subject><subject>Kinship</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Male animals</subject><subject>Males</subject><subject>Mating behavior</subject><subject>Microsatellite Repeats</subject><subject>Modeling</subject><subject>Models, Biological</subject><subject>Mountains</subject><subject>Paternity</subject><subject>Population Dynamics</subject><subject>Reproduction</subject><subject>Reproductive success</subject><subject>Sexual Behavior, Animal</subject><subject>Siblings</subject><subject>Social Dominance</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Species Specificity</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><issn>0027-8424</issn><issn>1091-6490</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2005</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0c2LEzEYBvAgittdPXsRHfYgXmb3zSSTSTwIUrQrdBF0xWPIZDJrajqp-Sj635vSslUPesohvzy8eR-EnmC4wNCRy82k4gW00AAWGJp7aIZB4JpRAffRDKDpak4beoJOY1wBgGg5PEQnuBWFcDpD-trnKSk7VQsfrHOqusm3tR_rLyq8qj5ZtzWhV_pbrK7U1lRLu7bJDNXcTyl4V_lyXX00m-CHrJP1U1WSrrNLdq2cqRbB5018hB6MykXz-HCeoc_v3t7Mr-rlh8X7-ZtlrVtBUt0TjAWh4zCwfiSsx5ppbJrBcDEKwrHgum2g18awgVAtiOqxoGo0LfB-wJScodf73E3u12bQpsyonNyEMkz4Kb2y8s-byX6Vt34rMWaMwS7gxSEg-O_ZxCTXNmpTtjIZn6NkneCcQvtfiDtGOui6As__giufw1S2IEtlpGO0ZQVd7pEOPsZgxruRMchdzXJXszzWXF48-_2nR3_otYDnB7B7eYxrZMOkoJgX8fLfQo7ZuWR-pEKf7ukqJh_uLCFdW6YhvwDmucaC</recordid><startdate>20050628</startdate><enddate>20050628</enddate><creator>Bradley, Brenda J.</creator><creator>Robbins, Martha M.</creator><creator>Williamson, Elizabeth A.</creator><creator>Steklis, H. Dieter</creator><creator>Steklis, Netzin Gerald</creator><creator>Eckhardt, Nadin</creator><creator>Boesch, Christophe</creator><creator>Vigilant, Linda</creator><creator>Frans B. M. de Waal</creator><general>National Academy of Sciences</general><general>National Acad Sciences</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7TO</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20050628</creationdate><title>Mountain Gorilla Tug-of-War: Silverbacks Have Limited Control over Reproduction in Multimale Groups</title><author>Bradley, Brenda J. ; Robbins, Martha M. ; Williamson, Elizabeth A. ; Steklis, H. Dieter ; Steklis, Netzin Gerald ; Eckhardt, Nadin ; Boesch, Christophe ; Vigilant, Linda ; Frans B. M. de Waal</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-b311934fdd6bf36b1c6c1e2de89f938198c520bcee6d34c93ab194afe508bd143</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2005</creationdate><topic>Alleles</topic><topic>Animal reproduction</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Behavior, Animal</topic><topic>Ecological modeling</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Genotype</topic><topic>Genotypes</topic><topic>Gorilla gorilla</topic><topic>Gorillas</topic><topic>Hierarchy, Social</topic><topic>Kinship</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Male animals</topic><topic>Males</topic><topic>Mating behavior</topic><topic>Microsatellite Repeats</topic><topic>Modeling</topic><topic>Models, Biological</topic><topic>Mountains</topic><topic>Paternity</topic><topic>Population Dynamics</topic><topic>Reproduction</topic><topic>Reproductive success</topic><topic>Sexual Behavior, Animal</topic><topic>Siblings</topic><topic>Social Dominance</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Species Specificity</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Bradley, Brenda J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Robbins, Martha M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Williamson, Elizabeth A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steklis, H. Dieter</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steklis, Netzin Gerald</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Eckhardt, Nadin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Boesch, Christophe</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vigilant, Linda</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Frans B. M. de Waal</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Bradley, Brenda J.</au><au>Robbins, Martha M.</au><au>Williamson, Elizabeth A.</au><au>Steklis, H. Dieter</au><au>Steklis, Netzin Gerald</au><au>Eckhardt, Nadin</au><au>Boesch, Christophe</au><au>Vigilant, Linda</au><au>Frans B. M. de Waal</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Mountain Gorilla Tug-of-War: Silverbacks Have Limited Control over Reproduction in Multimale Groups</atitle><jtitle>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS</jtitle><addtitle>Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A</addtitle><date>2005-06-28</date><risdate>2005</risdate><volume>102</volume><issue>26</issue><spage>9418</spage><epage>9423</epage><pages>9418-9423</pages><issn>0027-8424</issn><eissn>1091-6490</eissn><abstract>To determine who fathers the offspring in wild mountain gorilla groups containing more than one adult male silverback, we genotyped nearly one-fourth (n = 92) of the mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) living in the Virunga Volcanoes region of Africa. Paternity analysis of 48 offspring born into four groups between 1985 and 1999 revealed that, although all infants were sired by within-group males, the socially dominant silverback did not always monopolize reproduction within his group. Instead, the second-ranking male sired an average of 15% of group offspring. This result, in combination with previous findings that second-ranking males fare best by not leaving the group but by staying and waiting to assume dominance even if no reproduction is possible while waiting, is not consistent with expectations from a reproductive skew model in which the silverback concedes controllable reproduction to the second-ranking male. Instead, the data suggest a "tug-of-war" scenario in which neither the dominant nor the second-ranking male has full control over his relative reproductive share. The two top-ranked males were typically unrelated and this, in combination with the mixed paternity of group offspring, means that multimale gorilla groups do not approximate family groups. Instead, as long-term assemblages of related and unrelated individuals, gorilla groups are similar to chimpanzee groups and so offer interesting possibilities for kinbiased interactions among individuals.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>National Academy of Sciences</pub><pmid>15964984</pmid><doi>10.1073/pnas.0502019102</doi><tpages>6</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0027-8424 |
ispartof | Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 2005-06, Vol.102 (26), p.9418-9423 |
issn | 0027-8424 1091-6490 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_1166604 |
source | Jstor Complete Legacy; MEDLINE; PubMed Central; Alma/SFX Local Collection; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry |
subjects | Alleles Animal reproduction Animals Behavior, Animal Ecological modeling Female Genotype Genotypes Gorilla gorilla Gorillas Hierarchy, Social Kinship Male Male animals Males Mating behavior Microsatellite Repeats Modeling Models, Biological Mountains Paternity Population Dynamics Reproduction Reproductive success Sexual Behavior, Animal Siblings Social Dominance Social Sciences Species Specificity Time Factors |
title | Mountain Gorilla Tug-of-War: Silverbacks Have Limited Control over Reproduction in Multimale Groups |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-05T06%3A39%3A43IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Mountain%20Gorilla%20Tug-of-War:%20Silverbacks%20Have%20Limited%20Control%20over%20Reproduction%20in%20Multimale%20Groups&rft.jtitle=Proceedings%20of%20the%20National%20Academy%20of%20Sciences%20-%20PNAS&rft.au=Bradley,%20Brenda%20J.&rft.date=2005-06-28&rft.volume=102&rft.issue=26&rft.spage=9418&rft.epage=9423&rft.pages=9418-9423&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073/pnas.0502019102&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_pubme%3E3375910%3C/jstor_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=201376456&rft_id=info:pmid/15964984&rft_jstor_id=3375910&rfr_iscdi=true |