The use of formal language as a strong sign of verbal autistic children in diglossic communities: The case of Arabic

The current study aimed to investigate whether the use of formal language (Modern Standard Arabic [MSA]) by young children in diglossic Arab communities offers diagnostic insights, especially for verbal autistic children and to further explore this phenomenon. We used a cohort study design, with 4–6...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Autism research 2024-09, Vol.17 (12), p.2579-2587
Hauptverfasser: Francis, Konstantinos, Alshammari, Nasser, Alsulaihim, Nailah, Aboukhamseen, Suja, El Dardiri, Mohammad, AlRashidi, Fawzeiah, Ridha, Hashem Almutaz, AL‐Hassan, Mada, Terzi, Arhonto
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 2587
container_issue 12
container_start_page 2579
container_title Autism research
container_volume 17
creator Francis, Konstantinos
Alshammari, Nasser
Alsulaihim, Nailah
Aboukhamseen, Suja
El Dardiri, Mohammad
AlRashidi, Fawzeiah
Ridha, Hashem Almutaz
AL‐Hassan, Mada
Terzi, Arhonto
description The current study aimed to investigate whether the use of formal language (Modern Standard Arabic [MSA]) by young children in diglossic Arab communities offers diagnostic insights, especially for verbal autistic children and to further explore this phenomenon. We used a cohort study design, with 4–6‐year‐old fluent first language Arabic‐speaking children attending Arabic Kindergartens in two representative Kuwait governates. Reported cases for MSA use were assessed via a computer‐based structured language test and corroborated cases were further assessed for exposure to sources of MSA, verbal IQ, temperamental characteristics, and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Four children from the same class without developmental difficulties were selected for each MSA user as control group. The frequency of MSA use among verbal pre‐schoolers was 0.46%. Use of MSA did not correlate with parents' education, amount of exposure to MSA, verbal IQ, but with severity of ASD. Predicted probability of ASD in the presence of MSA was 0.86. Executive functions of ASD‐MSA users were similar to those of the control group and significantly higher than unselected autistic peers in the literature. The use of MSA has the potential to serve as a strong sign for the diagnosis of verbal autistic children, often missed or delayed in being diagnosed. We also discuss strategies via which language is acquired in ASD. Lay Summary Arabic is a language known to come in two clearly distinct varieties: one used in most everyday situations (colloquial) and another (formal) used in formal settings, such as school books and teaching, religious settings, video games, Arab cartoons, TV news, and documentaries. We noticed that children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may use the formal version in everyday interactions, which is highly uncommon. Our study aimed to see if this uncommon use of formal language could lead to early detection of ASD. We examined 4–6‐year‐old children in Kuwaiti public kindergartens, checking for signs of ASD in those children who used formal language in their daily chats. Out of 5314 children, only a few (0.46%) used formal language in this context, and most of them (86%) were diagnosed for ASD. This odd language use wasn't linked to children's intelligence, to their parents' education level, or to how much they watched TV or used electronics. Instead, it correlated with the social and communication challenges of ASD. Our findings suggest that noticing such language differ
doi_str_mv 10.1002/aur.3237
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11638891</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>3143695874</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c2967-ae7725bada949dfee6fde46e4ffcde14e50cb5efc4f0a8d85e9c0dac39a201783</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kV1r1jAYhoMobk7BXyABTzzpTJqmbTyRl-EXDATZjsPT5EnfjLaZSTPZvzf13eYHeJSQXFzPx03IS85OOWP1W8jxVNSie0SOuRKqEj1rH9_fO1UfkWcpXTHWMiHrp-RIKMGY4uqYrBd7pDkhDY66EGeY6ATLmGFECokCTWsMy0iTH5eNucE4FAby6tPqDTV7P9mIC_ULtX6cQkrba5jnvPjVY3pHtwoGDiV2EQZvnpMnDqaEL-7OE3L58cPF2efq_OunL2e788rUqu0qwK6r5QAWVKOsQ2ydxabFxjljkTcomRkkOtM4Br3tJSrDLBihoGa868UJeX_wXudhRmtwWSNM-jr6GeKtDuD13z-L3-sx3GjOW9H3ihfDmztDDN8zplXPPhmcyo4w5KQFZx2XvaxlQV__g16FHJcyX6Ea0SrZd81voYllVRHdQzec6S1LXbLUW5YFffVn9w_gfXgFqA7ADz_h7X9Fenf57ZfwJ_g2qtE</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>3143695874</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The use of formal language as a strong sign of verbal autistic children in diglossic communities: The case of Arabic</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via Wiley Online Library</source><creator>Francis, Konstantinos ; Alshammari, Nasser ; Alsulaihim, Nailah ; Aboukhamseen, Suja ; El Dardiri, Mohammad ; AlRashidi, Fawzeiah ; Ridha, Hashem Almutaz ; AL‐Hassan, Mada ; Terzi, Arhonto</creator><creatorcontrib>Francis, Konstantinos ; Alshammari, Nasser ; Alsulaihim, Nailah ; Aboukhamseen, Suja ; El Dardiri, Mohammad ; AlRashidi, Fawzeiah ; Ridha, Hashem Almutaz ; AL‐Hassan, Mada ; Terzi, Arhonto</creatorcontrib><description>The current study aimed to investigate whether the use of formal language (Modern Standard Arabic [MSA]) by young children in diglossic Arab communities offers diagnostic insights, especially for verbal autistic children and to further explore this phenomenon. We used a cohort study design, with 4–6‐year‐old fluent first language Arabic‐speaking children attending Arabic Kindergartens in two representative Kuwait governates. Reported cases for MSA use were assessed via a computer‐based structured language test and corroborated cases were further assessed for exposure to sources of MSA, verbal IQ, temperamental characteristics, and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Four children from the same class without developmental difficulties were selected for each MSA user as control group. The frequency of MSA use among verbal pre‐schoolers was 0.46%. Use of MSA did not correlate with parents' education, amount of exposure to MSA, verbal IQ, but with severity of ASD. Predicted probability of ASD in the presence of MSA was 0.86. Executive functions of ASD‐MSA users were similar to those of the control group and significantly higher than unselected autistic peers in the literature. The use of MSA has the potential to serve as a strong sign for the diagnosis of verbal autistic children, often missed or delayed in being diagnosed. We also discuss strategies via which language is acquired in ASD. Lay Summary Arabic is a language known to come in two clearly distinct varieties: one used in most everyday situations (colloquial) and another (formal) used in formal settings, such as school books and teaching, religious settings, video games, Arab cartoons, TV news, and documentaries. We noticed that children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may use the formal version in everyday interactions, which is highly uncommon. Our study aimed to see if this uncommon use of formal language could lead to early detection of ASD. We examined 4–6‐year‐old children in Kuwaiti public kindergartens, checking for signs of ASD in those children who used formal language in their daily chats. Out of 5314 children, only a few (0.46%) used formal language in this context, and most of them (86%) were diagnosed for ASD. This odd language use wasn't linked to children's intelligence, to their parents' education level, or to how much they watched TV or used electronics. Instead, it correlated with the social and communication challenges of ASD. Our findings suggest that noticing such language differences can help identify verbal autistic children who might otherwise be missed in diagnosis. This can lead to earlier support, but also offer insights into how these children learn language differently, hence, potentially improve intervention.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1939-3792</identifier><identifier>ISSN: 1939-3806</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-3806</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1002/aur.3237</identifier><identifier>PMID: 39300919</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Hoboken, USA: John Wiley &amp; Sons, Inc</publisher><subject>Arabs - psychology ; Arabs - statistics &amp; numerical data ; ASD screening ; Autism ; Autism Spectrum Disorder - diagnosis ; Autistic children ; Autistic Disorder - complications ; Child ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Cohort Studies ; diglossia ; Female ; Humans ; idiosyncratic language ; Intelligence ; Kindergarten ; Kuwait ; Language ; language acquisition ; Male ; Predictive control ; PSYCHOLOGY</subject><ispartof>Autism research, 2024-09, Vol.17 (12), p.2579-2587</ispartof><rights>2024 The Author(s). published by International Society for Autism Research and Wiley Periodicals LLC.</rights><rights>2024 The Author(s). Autism Research published by International Society for Autism Research and Wiley Periodicals LLC.</rights><rights>2024. This article is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><rights>2024 International Society for Autism Research and Wiley Periodicals LLC.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c2967-ae7725bada949dfee6fde46e4ffcde14e50cb5efc4f0a8d85e9c0dac39a201783</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-1352-1732</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002%2Faur.3237$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002%2Faur.3237$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,1417,27924,27925,45574,45575</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/39300919$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Francis, Konstantinos</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alshammari, Nasser</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alsulaihim, Nailah</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Aboukhamseen, Suja</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>El Dardiri, Mohammad</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>AlRashidi, Fawzeiah</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ridha, Hashem Almutaz</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>AL‐Hassan, Mada</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Terzi, Arhonto</creatorcontrib><title>The use of formal language as a strong sign of verbal autistic children in diglossic communities: The case of Arabic</title><title>Autism research</title><addtitle>Autism Res</addtitle><description>The current study aimed to investigate whether the use of formal language (Modern Standard Arabic [MSA]) by young children in diglossic Arab communities offers diagnostic insights, especially for verbal autistic children and to further explore this phenomenon. We used a cohort study design, with 4–6‐year‐old fluent first language Arabic‐speaking children attending Arabic Kindergartens in two representative Kuwait governates. Reported cases for MSA use were assessed via a computer‐based structured language test and corroborated cases were further assessed for exposure to sources of MSA, verbal IQ, temperamental characteristics, and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Four children from the same class without developmental difficulties were selected for each MSA user as control group. The frequency of MSA use among verbal pre‐schoolers was 0.46%. Use of MSA did not correlate with parents' education, amount of exposure to MSA, verbal IQ, but with severity of ASD. Predicted probability of ASD in the presence of MSA was 0.86. Executive functions of ASD‐MSA users were similar to those of the control group and significantly higher than unselected autistic peers in the literature. The use of MSA has the potential to serve as a strong sign for the diagnosis of verbal autistic children, often missed or delayed in being diagnosed. We also discuss strategies via which language is acquired in ASD. Lay Summary Arabic is a language known to come in two clearly distinct varieties: one used in most everyday situations (colloquial) and another (formal) used in formal settings, such as school books and teaching, religious settings, video games, Arab cartoons, TV news, and documentaries. We noticed that children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may use the formal version in everyday interactions, which is highly uncommon. Our study aimed to see if this uncommon use of formal language could lead to early detection of ASD. We examined 4–6‐year‐old children in Kuwaiti public kindergartens, checking for signs of ASD in those children who used formal language in their daily chats. Out of 5314 children, only a few (0.46%) used formal language in this context, and most of them (86%) were diagnosed for ASD. This odd language use wasn't linked to children's intelligence, to their parents' education level, or to how much they watched TV or used electronics. Instead, it correlated with the social and communication challenges of ASD. Our findings suggest that noticing such language differences can help identify verbal autistic children who might otherwise be missed in diagnosis. This can lead to earlier support, but also offer insights into how these children learn language differently, hence, potentially improve intervention.</description><subject>Arabs - psychology</subject><subject>Arabs - statistics &amp; numerical data</subject><subject>ASD screening</subject><subject>Autism</subject><subject>Autism Spectrum Disorder - diagnosis</subject><subject>Autistic children</subject><subject>Autistic Disorder - complications</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Cohort Studies</subject><subject>diglossia</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>idiosyncratic language</subject><subject>Intelligence</subject><subject>Kindergarten</subject><subject>Kuwait</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>language acquisition</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Predictive control</subject><subject>PSYCHOLOGY</subject><issn>1939-3792</issn><issn>1939-3806</issn><issn>1939-3806</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2024</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>24P</sourceid><sourceid>WIN</sourceid><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kV1r1jAYhoMobk7BXyABTzzpTJqmbTyRl-EXDATZjsPT5EnfjLaZSTPZvzf13eYHeJSQXFzPx03IS85OOWP1W8jxVNSie0SOuRKqEj1rH9_fO1UfkWcpXTHWMiHrp-RIKMGY4uqYrBd7pDkhDY66EGeY6ATLmGFECokCTWsMy0iTH5eNucE4FAby6tPqDTV7P9mIC_ULtX6cQkrba5jnvPjVY3pHtwoGDiV2EQZvnpMnDqaEL-7OE3L58cPF2efq_OunL2e788rUqu0qwK6r5QAWVKOsQ2ydxabFxjljkTcomRkkOtM4Br3tJSrDLBihoGa868UJeX_wXudhRmtwWSNM-jr6GeKtDuD13z-L3-sx3GjOW9H3ihfDmztDDN8zplXPPhmcyo4w5KQFZx2XvaxlQV__g16FHJcyX6Ea0SrZd81voYllVRHdQzec6S1LXbLUW5YFffVn9w_gfXgFqA7ADz_h7X9Fenf57ZfwJ_g2qtE</recordid><startdate>20240920</startdate><enddate>20240920</enddate><creator>Francis, Konstantinos</creator><creator>Alshammari, Nasser</creator><creator>Alsulaihim, Nailah</creator><creator>Aboukhamseen, Suja</creator><creator>El Dardiri, Mohammad</creator><creator>AlRashidi, Fawzeiah</creator><creator>Ridha, Hashem Almutaz</creator><creator>AL‐Hassan, Mada</creator><creator>Terzi, Arhonto</creator><general>John Wiley &amp; Sons, Inc</general><general>Wiley Subscription Services, Inc</general><scope>24P</scope><scope>WIN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1352-1732</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20240920</creationdate><title>The use of formal language as a strong sign of verbal autistic children in diglossic communities: The case of Arabic</title><author>Francis, Konstantinos ; Alshammari, Nasser ; Alsulaihim, Nailah ; Aboukhamseen, Suja ; El Dardiri, Mohammad ; AlRashidi, Fawzeiah ; Ridha, Hashem Almutaz ; AL‐Hassan, Mada ; Terzi, Arhonto</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c2967-ae7725bada949dfee6fde46e4ffcde14e50cb5efc4f0a8d85e9c0dac39a201783</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2024</creationdate><topic>Arabs - psychology</topic><topic>Arabs - statistics &amp; numerical data</topic><topic>ASD screening</topic><topic>Autism</topic><topic>Autism Spectrum Disorder - diagnosis</topic><topic>Autistic children</topic><topic>Autistic Disorder - complications</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Cohort Studies</topic><topic>diglossia</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>idiosyncratic language</topic><topic>Intelligence</topic><topic>Kindergarten</topic><topic>Kuwait</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>language acquisition</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Predictive control</topic><topic>PSYCHOLOGY</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Francis, Konstantinos</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alshammari, Nasser</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alsulaihim, Nailah</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Aboukhamseen, Suja</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>El Dardiri, Mohammad</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>AlRashidi, Fawzeiah</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ridha, Hashem Almutaz</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>AL‐Hassan, Mada</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Terzi, Arhonto</creatorcontrib><collection>Wiley Online Library (Open Access Collection)</collection><collection>Wiley Online Library Free Content</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Autism research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Francis, Konstantinos</au><au>Alshammari, Nasser</au><au>Alsulaihim, Nailah</au><au>Aboukhamseen, Suja</au><au>El Dardiri, Mohammad</au><au>AlRashidi, Fawzeiah</au><au>Ridha, Hashem Almutaz</au><au>AL‐Hassan, Mada</au><au>Terzi, Arhonto</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The use of formal language as a strong sign of verbal autistic children in diglossic communities: The case of Arabic</atitle><jtitle>Autism research</jtitle><addtitle>Autism Res</addtitle><date>2024-09-20</date><risdate>2024</risdate><volume>17</volume><issue>12</issue><spage>2579</spage><epage>2587</epage><pages>2579-2587</pages><issn>1939-3792</issn><issn>1939-3806</issn><eissn>1939-3806</eissn><abstract>The current study aimed to investigate whether the use of formal language (Modern Standard Arabic [MSA]) by young children in diglossic Arab communities offers diagnostic insights, especially for verbal autistic children and to further explore this phenomenon. We used a cohort study design, with 4–6‐year‐old fluent first language Arabic‐speaking children attending Arabic Kindergartens in two representative Kuwait governates. Reported cases for MSA use were assessed via a computer‐based structured language test and corroborated cases were further assessed for exposure to sources of MSA, verbal IQ, temperamental characteristics, and autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Four children from the same class without developmental difficulties were selected for each MSA user as control group. The frequency of MSA use among verbal pre‐schoolers was 0.46%. Use of MSA did not correlate with parents' education, amount of exposure to MSA, verbal IQ, but with severity of ASD. Predicted probability of ASD in the presence of MSA was 0.86. Executive functions of ASD‐MSA users were similar to those of the control group and significantly higher than unselected autistic peers in the literature. The use of MSA has the potential to serve as a strong sign for the diagnosis of verbal autistic children, often missed or delayed in being diagnosed. We also discuss strategies via which language is acquired in ASD. Lay Summary Arabic is a language known to come in two clearly distinct varieties: one used in most everyday situations (colloquial) and another (formal) used in formal settings, such as school books and teaching, religious settings, video games, Arab cartoons, TV news, and documentaries. We noticed that children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may use the formal version in everyday interactions, which is highly uncommon. Our study aimed to see if this uncommon use of formal language could lead to early detection of ASD. We examined 4–6‐year‐old children in Kuwaiti public kindergartens, checking for signs of ASD in those children who used formal language in their daily chats. Out of 5314 children, only a few (0.46%) used formal language in this context, and most of them (86%) were diagnosed for ASD. This odd language use wasn't linked to children's intelligence, to their parents' education level, or to how much they watched TV or used electronics. Instead, it correlated with the social and communication challenges of ASD. Our findings suggest that noticing such language differences can help identify verbal autistic children who might otherwise be missed in diagnosis. This can lead to earlier support, but also offer insights into how these children learn language differently, hence, potentially improve intervention.</abstract><cop>Hoboken, USA</cop><pub>John Wiley &amp; Sons, Inc</pub><pmid>39300919</pmid><doi>10.1002/aur.3237</doi><tpages>9</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1352-1732</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1939-3792
ispartof Autism research, 2024-09, Vol.17 (12), p.2579-2587
issn 1939-3792
1939-3806
1939-3806
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11638891
source MEDLINE; Access via Wiley Online Library
subjects Arabs - psychology
Arabs - statistics & numerical data
ASD screening
Autism
Autism Spectrum Disorder - diagnosis
Autistic children
Autistic Disorder - complications
Child
Child, Preschool
Children
Cohort Studies
diglossia
Female
Humans
idiosyncratic language
Intelligence
Kindergarten
Kuwait
Language
language acquisition
Male
Predictive control
PSYCHOLOGY
title The use of formal language as a strong sign of verbal autistic children in diglossic communities: The case of Arabic
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-22T21%3A04%3A37IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20use%20of%20formal%20language%20as%20a%20strong%20sign%20of%20verbal%20autistic%20children%20in%20diglossic%20communities:%20The%20case%20of%20Arabic&rft.jtitle=Autism%20research&rft.au=Francis,%20Konstantinos&rft.date=2024-09-20&rft.volume=17&rft.issue=12&rft.spage=2579&rft.epage=2587&rft.pages=2579-2587&rft.issn=1939-3792&rft.eissn=1939-3806&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002/aur.3237&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E3143695874%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=3143695874&rft_id=info:pmid/39300919&rfr_iscdi=true