Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict

Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range fr...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Attention, perception & psychophysics perception & psychophysics, 2024-04, Vol.86 (3), p.750-767
Hauptverfasser: Strivens, Amy, Koch, Iring, Lavric, Aureliu
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 767
container_issue 3
container_start_page 750
container_title Attention, perception & psychophysics
container_volume 86
creator Strivens, Amy
Koch, Iring
Lavric, Aureliu
description Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.
doi_str_mv 10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11062987</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2929027588</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kc1u1DAUhSMEoj_wAiyQJTZsAr6-Gcdmg6rSAlIlNiCxsxzH6bjK2MF2ppo34LHxNO3ws2Bh2dL97jn3-lTVC6BvsF2JtwmwgaamDMsRDdS7R9UxyAZrlPj98eHN4Kg6SemGUo68pU-rIxQMWNOK4-rnh2ATmaKddNTZBU_WdpxIDiTdumzWRM-9yyHuiM7Z-juis_nWWk-S28xj1t6GOZFtcMamd-RiGKzJiYThQWGKodOdG10uIr7fm231aL2xe8gEP4zO5GfVk0GPyT6_v0-rb5cXX88_1VdfPn4-P7uqTdOuci0l55oNXcO45BoRGe81QvFseQPUdGJlYNVJPSBwOfQUhWCyA2gMSqCIp9X7RXeau43tTdkp6lFN0W103Kmgnfq74t1aXYetAqCcSdEWhdf3CjH8mG3KauOSseO4_IRikknKSkCioK_-QW_CHH3ZTyFdAQdBBSsUWygTQ0rRDodpgKp90mpJWpWk1V3SaleaXv65x6HlIdoC4AKkUvLXNv72_o_sL-yat-I</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>3051618082</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>Strivens, Amy ; Koch, Iring ; Lavric, Aureliu</creator><creatorcontrib>Strivens, Amy ; Koch, Iring ; Lavric, Aureliu</creatorcontrib><description>Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1943-3921</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1943-393X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y</identifier><identifier>PMID: 38212478</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Springer US</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Attention ; Auditory Perception ; Behavioral Science and Psychology ; Cognitive Psychology ; Conflict, Psychological ; Costs ; Cues ; Experiments ; Familiarity ; Feedback (Response) ; Female ; Gender ; Humans ; Male ; Probability ; Psychology ; Reaction Time ; Repetition ; Speech Perception ; Stimuli ; Voice ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics, 2024-04, Vol.86 (3), p.750-767</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2024</rights><rights>2024. The Author(s).</rights><rights>Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Apr 2024</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925,41488,42557,51319</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38212478$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Strivens, Amy</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Koch, Iring</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lavric, Aureliu</creatorcontrib><title>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</title><title>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics</title><addtitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</addtitle><addtitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</addtitle><description>Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Auditory Perception</subject><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology</subject><subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject><subject>Conflict, Psychological</subject><subject>Costs</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Familiarity</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Gender</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Probability</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Repetition</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><subject>Stimuli</subject><subject>Voice</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1943-3921</issn><issn>1943-393X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2024</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>C6C</sourceid><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kc1u1DAUhSMEoj_wAiyQJTZsAr6-Gcdmg6rSAlIlNiCxsxzH6bjK2MF2ppo34LHxNO3ws2Bh2dL97jn3-lTVC6BvsF2JtwmwgaamDMsRDdS7R9UxyAZrlPj98eHN4Kg6SemGUo68pU-rIxQMWNOK4-rnh2ATmaKddNTZBU_WdpxIDiTdumzWRM-9yyHuiM7Z-juis_nWWk-S28xj1t6GOZFtcMamd-RiGKzJiYThQWGKodOdG10uIr7fm231aL2xe8gEP4zO5GfVk0GPyT6_v0-rb5cXX88_1VdfPn4-P7uqTdOuci0l55oNXcO45BoRGe81QvFseQPUdGJlYNVJPSBwOfQUhWCyA2gMSqCIp9X7RXeau43tTdkp6lFN0W103Kmgnfq74t1aXYetAqCcSdEWhdf3CjH8mG3KauOSseO4_IRikknKSkCioK_-QW_CHH3ZTyFdAQdBBSsUWygTQ0rRDodpgKp90mpJWpWk1V3SaleaXv65x6HlIdoC4AKkUvLXNv72_o_sL-yat-I</recordid><startdate>20240401</startdate><enddate>20240401</enddate><creator>Strivens, Amy</creator><creator>Koch, Iring</creator><creator>Lavric, Aureliu</creator><general>Springer US</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20240401</creationdate><title>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</title><author>Strivens, Amy ; Koch, Iring ; Lavric, Aureliu</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2024</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Auditory Perception</topic><topic>Behavioral Science and Psychology</topic><topic>Cognitive Psychology</topic><topic>Conflict, Psychological</topic><topic>Costs</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Familiarity</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Gender</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Probability</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Repetition</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><topic>Stimuli</topic><topic>Voice</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Strivens, Amy</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Koch, Iring</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lavric, Aureliu</creatorcontrib><collection>Springer Nature OA Free Journals</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Strivens, Amy</au><au>Koch, Iring</au><au>Lavric, Aureliu</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</atitle><jtitle>Attention, perception &amp; psychophysics</jtitle><stitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</stitle><addtitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</addtitle><date>2024-04-01</date><risdate>2024</risdate><volume>86</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>750</spage><epage>767</epage><pages>750-767</pages><issn>1943-3921</issn><eissn>1943-393X</eissn><abstract>Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Springer US</pub><pmid>38212478</pmid><doi>10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y</doi><tpages>18</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1943-3921
ispartof Attention, perception & psychophysics, 2024-04, Vol.86 (3), p.750-767
issn 1943-3921
1943-393X
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11062987
source MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings
subjects Adolescent
Adult
Attention
Auditory Perception
Behavioral Science and Psychology
Cognitive Psychology
Conflict, Psychological
Costs
Cues
Experiments
Familiarity
Feedback (Response)
Female
Gender
Humans
Male
Probability
Psychology
Reaction Time
Repetition
Speech Perception
Stimuli
Voice
Young Adult
title Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-19T18%3A07%3A04IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Does%20preparation%20help%20to%20switch%20auditory%20attention%20between%20simultaneous%20voices:%20Effects%20of%20switch%20probability%20and%20prevalence%20of%20conflict&rft.jtitle=Attention,%20perception%20&%20psychophysics&rft.au=Strivens,%20Amy&rft.date=2024-04-01&rft.volume=86&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=750&rft.epage=767&rft.pages=750-767&rft.issn=1943-3921&rft.eissn=1943-393X&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2929027588%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=3051618082&rft_id=info:pmid/38212478&rfr_iscdi=true