Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict
Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range fr...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Attention, perception & psychophysics perception & psychophysics, 2024-04, Vol.86 (3), p.750-767 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 767 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 750 |
container_title | Attention, perception & psychophysics |
container_volume | 86 |
creator | Strivens, Amy Koch, Iring Lavric, Aureliu |
description | Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice. |
doi_str_mv | 10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11062987</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2929027588</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kc1u1DAUhSMEoj_wAiyQJTZsAr6-Gcdmg6rSAlIlNiCxsxzH6bjK2MF2ppo34LHxNO3ws2Bh2dL97jn3-lTVC6BvsF2JtwmwgaamDMsRDdS7R9UxyAZrlPj98eHN4Kg6SemGUo68pU-rIxQMWNOK4-rnh2ATmaKddNTZBU_WdpxIDiTdumzWRM-9yyHuiM7Z-juis_nWWk-S28xj1t6GOZFtcMamd-RiGKzJiYThQWGKodOdG10uIr7fm231aL2xe8gEP4zO5GfVk0GPyT6_v0-rb5cXX88_1VdfPn4-P7uqTdOuci0l55oNXcO45BoRGe81QvFseQPUdGJlYNVJPSBwOfQUhWCyA2gMSqCIp9X7RXeau43tTdkp6lFN0W103Kmgnfq74t1aXYetAqCcSdEWhdf3CjH8mG3KauOSseO4_IRikknKSkCioK_-QW_CHH3ZTyFdAQdBBSsUWygTQ0rRDodpgKp90mpJWpWk1V3SaleaXv65x6HlIdoC4AKkUvLXNv72_o_sL-yat-I</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>3051618082</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>Strivens, Amy ; Koch, Iring ; Lavric, Aureliu</creator><creatorcontrib>Strivens, Amy ; Koch, Iring ; Lavric, Aureliu</creatorcontrib><description>Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1943-3921</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1943-393X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y</identifier><identifier>PMID: 38212478</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Springer US</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Attention ; Auditory Perception ; Behavioral Science and Psychology ; Cognitive Psychology ; Conflict, Psychological ; Costs ; Cues ; Experiments ; Familiarity ; Feedback (Response) ; Female ; Gender ; Humans ; Male ; Probability ; Psychology ; Reaction Time ; Repetition ; Speech Perception ; Stimuli ; Voice ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Attention, perception & psychophysics, 2024-04, Vol.86 (3), p.750-767</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2024</rights><rights>2024. The Author(s).</rights><rights>Copyright Springer Nature B.V. Apr 2024</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925,41488,42557,51319</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/38212478$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Strivens, Amy</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Koch, Iring</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lavric, Aureliu</creatorcontrib><title>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</title><title>Attention, perception & psychophysics</title><addtitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</addtitle><addtitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</addtitle><description>Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Auditory Perception</subject><subject>Behavioral Science and Psychology</subject><subject>Cognitive Psychology</subject><subject>Conflict, Psychological</subject><subject>Costs</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Familiarity</subject><subject>Feedback (Response)</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Gender</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Probability</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><subject>Repetition</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><subject>Stimuli</subject><subject>Voice</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1943-3921</issn><issn>1943-393X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2024</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>C6C</sourceid><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kc1u1DAUhSMEoj_wAiyQJTZsAr6-Gcdmg6rSAlIlNiCxsxzH6bjK2MF2ppo34LHxNO3ws2Bh2dL97jn3-lTVC6BvsF2JtwmwgaamDMsRDdS7R9UxyAZrlPj98eHN4Kg6SemGUo68pU-rIxQMWNOK4-rnh2ATmaKddNTZBU_WdpxIDiTdumzWRM-9yyHuiM7Z-juis_nWWk-S28xj1t6GOZFtcMamd-RiGKzJiYThQWGKodOdG10uIr7fm231aL2xe8gEP4zO5GfVk0GPyT6_v0-rb5cXX88_1VdfPn4-P7uqTdOuci0l55oNXcO45BoRGe81QvFseQPUdGJlYNVJPSBwOfQUhWCyA2gMSqCIp9X7RXeau43tTdkp6lFN0W103Kmgnfq74t1aXYetAqCcSdEWhdf3CjH8mG3KauOSseO4_IRikknKSkCioK_-QW_CHH3ZTyFdAQdBBSsUWygTQ0rRDodpgKp90mpJWpWk1V3SaleaXv65x6HlIdoC4AKkUvLXNv72_o_sL-yat-I</recordid><startdate>20240401</startdate><enddate>20240401</enddate><creator>Strivens, Amy</creator><creator>Koch, Iring</creator><creator>Lavric, Aureliu</creator><general>Springer US</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>4T-</scope><scope>4U-</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20240401</creationdate><title>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</title><author>Strivens, Amy ; Koch, Iring ; Lavric, Aureliu</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c475t-9966a2fb42696a33326da31fec76410cb85c15b9af3169fd038829b114c391033</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2024</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Auditory Perception</topic><topic>Behavioral Science and Psychology</topic><topic>Cognitive Psychology</topic><topic>Conflict, Psychological</topic><topic>Costs</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Familiarity</topic><topic>Feedback (Response)</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Gender</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Probability</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><topic>Repetition</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><topic>Stimuli</topic><topic>Voice</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Strivens, Amy</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Koch, Iring</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Lavric, Aureliu</creatorcontrib><collection>Springer Nature OA Free Journals</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Docstoc</collection><collection>University Readers</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Attention, perception & psychophysics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Strivens, Amy</au><au>Koch, Iring</au><au>Lavric, Aureliu</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict</atitle><jtitle>Attention, perception & psychophysics</jtitle><stitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</stitle><addtitle>Atten Percept Psychophys</addtitle><date>2024-04-01</date><risdate>2024</risdate><volume>86</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>750</spage><epage>767</epage><pages>750-767</pages><issn>1943-3921</issn><eissn>1943-393X</eissn><abstract>Switching auditory attention to one of two (or more) simultaneous voices incurs a substantial performance overhead. Whether/when this voice ‘switch cost’ reduces when the listener has opportunity to prepare in silence is not clear–the findings on the effect of preparation on the switch cost range from (near) null to substantial. We sought to determine which factors are crucial for encouraging preparation and detecting its effect on the switch cost in a paradigm where participants categorized the number spoken by one of two simultaneous voices; the target voice, which changed unpredictably, was specified by a visual cue depicting the target’s gender. First, we manipulated the probability of a voice switch. When 25% of trials were switches, increasing the preparation interval (50/800/1,400 ms) resulted in substantial (~50%) reduction in switch cost. No reduction was observed when 75% of trials were switches. Second, we examined the relative prevalence of low-conflict, ‘congruent’ trials (where the numbers spoken by the two voices were mapped onto the same response) and high-conflict, ‘incongruent’ trials (where the voices afforded different responses). ‘Conflict prevalence’ had a strong effect on selectivity–the incongruent–congruent difference (‘congruence effect’) was reduced in the 66%-incongruent condition relative to the 66%-congruent condition–but conflict prevalence did not discernibly interact with preparation and its effect on the switch cost. Thus, conditions where switches of target voice are relatively rare are especially conducive to preparation, possibly because attention is committed more strongly to (and/or disengaged less rapidly from) the perceptual features of target voice.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Springer US</pub><pmid>38212478</pmid><doi>10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y</doi><tpages>18</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1943-3921 |
ispartof | Attention, perception & psychophysics, 2024-04, Vol.86 (3), p.750-767 |
issn | 1943-3921 1943-393X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_11062987 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Attention Auditory Perception Behavioral Science and Psychology Cognitive Psychology Conflict, Psychological Costs Cues Experiments Familiarity Feedback (Response) Female Gender Humans Male Probability Psychology Reaction Time Repetition Speech Perception Stimuli Voice Young Adult |
title | Does preparation help to switch auditory attention between simultaneous voices: Effects of switch probability and prevalence of conflict |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-19T18%3A07%3A04IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Does%20preparation%20help%20to%20switch%20auditory%20attention%20between%20simultaneous%20voices:%20Effects%20of%20switch%20probability%20and%20prevalence%20of%20conflict&rft.jtitle=Attention,%20perception%20&%20psychophysics&rft.au=Strivens,%20Amy&rft.date=2024-04-01&rft.volume=86&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=750&rft.epage=767&rft.pages=750-767&rft.issn=1943-3921&rft.eissn=1943-393X&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758/s13414-023-02841-y&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2929027588%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=3051618082&rft_id=info:pmid/38212478&rfr_iscdi=true |