Continuous use of visual information about the position of the moving hand
People generally look at a target when they want to reach for it. Doing so presumably helps them continuously update their judgments about the target’s position and motion. But not looking at their hand does not prevent people from updating judgments about its position on the basis of visual informa...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Experimental brain research 2023-08, Vol.241 (8), p.2001-2008 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 2008 |
---|---|
container_issue | 8 |
container_start_page | 2001 |
container_title | Experimental brain research |
container_volume | 241 |
creator | Brenner, Eli Smeets, Jeroen B. J. |
description | People generally look at a target when they want to reach for it. Doing so presumably helps them continuously update their judgments about the target’s position and motion. But not looking at their hand does not prevent people from updating judgments about its position on the basis of visual information, because people do respond to experimental perturbations of visual information about the position of their hand. Here, we study such responses by adding jitter to the movement of a cursor that follows participants’ fingers. We analyse the response to the jitter in a way that reveals how the vigour of the response depends on the moment during the movement at which the change in cursor position occurs. We compare the change in vigour to that for equivalent jitter in the position of the target. We find that participants respond to jitter in the position of a cursor in much the same way as they respond to jitter in the target’s position. The responses are more vigorous late in the movement, when adjustments need to be made within less time, but similarly so for the cursor as for the target. The responses are weaker for the cursor, presumably because of the jitter-free kinaesthetic information about the position of the finger. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00221-023-06658-x |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_10386923</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2843477051</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-9745fb50334949ce3454b30803ee054c2ce93516ed54a515a95fa844c8c44c083</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kUtPAyEUhYnRaH38ARdmEjduRi-vmWFlTOMzJm50TShlWpoZqDDT1H8vtfW5cAO53O8eOByEjjGcY4DyIgIQgnMgNIei4FW-3EIDzCjJMYZiGw0AMMtZhcUe2o9xtippCbtoj5a0KrDgA_Qw9K6zrvd9zPpoMl9nCxt71WTW1T60qrPeZWrk-y7rpiab-2g_jhK4qlu_sG6STZUbH6KdWjXRHG32A_Ryc_08vMsfn27vh1ePuWak6HJRMl6POFDKBBPaUMbZiEIF1BjgTBNtBOW4MGPOFMdcCV6rijFd6bRARQ_Q5Vp33o9aM9bGdUE1ch5sq8Kb9MrK3x1np3LiFxJDci0ITQpnG4XgX3sTO9naqE3TKGfSR0hSUcJLAaxM6OkfdOb74JK_RDHKyhI4ThRZUzr4GIOpv16DQa6ykuusZMpKfmQll2no5KePr5HPcBJA10BMLTcx4fvuf2TfAS3tn34</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2843477051</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Continuous use of visual information about the position of the moving hand</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>Brenner, Eli ; Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</creator><creatorcontrib>Brenner, Eli ; Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</creatorcontrib><description>People generally look at a target when they want to reach for it. Doing so presumably helps them continuously update their judgments about the target’s position and motion. But not looking at their hand does not prevent people from updating judgments about its position on the basis of visual information, because people do respond to experimental perturbations of visual information about the position of their hand. Here, we study such responses by adding jitter to the movement of a cursor that follows participants’ fingers. We analyse the response to the jitter in a way that reveals how the vigour of the response depends on the moment during the movement at which the change in cursor position occurs. We compare the change in vigour to that for equivalent jitter in the position of the target. We find that participants respond to jitter in the position of a cursor in much the same way as they respond to jitter in the target’s position. The responses are more vigorous late in the movement, when adjustments need to be made within less time, but similarly so for the cursor as for the target. The responses are weaker for the cursor, presumably because of the jitter-free kinaesthetic information about the position of the finger.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-023-06658-x</identifier><identifier>PMID: 37386195</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain research ; Fingers ; Hand ; Hand - physiology ; Humans ; Motion Perception - physiology ; Movement - physiology ; Neurology ; Neurosciences ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Research Article</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2023-08, Vol.241 (8), p.2001-2008</ispartof><rights>The Author(s) 2023</rights><rights>2023. The Author(s).</rights><rights>The Author(s) 2023. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-9745fb50334949ce3454b30803ee054c2ce93516ed54a515a95fa844c8c44c083</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-3611-2843</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00221-023-06658-x$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00221-023-06658-x$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$Hfree_for_read</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,776,780,881,27901,27902,41464,42533,51294</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37386195$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Brenner, Eli</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</creatorcontrib><title>Continuous use of visual information about the position of the moving hand</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>People generally look at a target when they want to reach for it. Doing so presumably helps them continuously update their judgments about the target’s position and motion. But not looking at their hand does not prevent people from updating judgments about its position on the basis of visual information, because people do respond to experimental perturbations of visual information about the position of their hand. Here, we study such responses by adding jitter to the movement of a cursor that follows participants’ fingers. We analyse the response to the jitter in a way that reveals how the vigour of the response depends on the moment during the movement at which the change in cursor position occurs. We compare the change in vigour to that for equivalent jitter in the position of the target. We find that participants respond to jitter in the position of a cursor in much the same way as they respond to jitter in the target’s position. The responses are more vigorous late in the movement, when adjustments need to be made within less time, but similarly so for the cursor as for the target. The responses are weaker for the cursor, presumably because of the jitter-free kinaesthetic information about the position of the finger.</description><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain research</subject><subject>Fingers</subject><subject>Hand</subject><subject>Hand - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Motion Perception - physiology</subject><subject>Movement - physiology</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Research Article</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>C6C</sourceid><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kUtPAyEUhYnRaH38ARdmEjduRi-vmWFlTOMzJm50TShlWpoZqDDT1H8vtfW5cAO53O8eOByEjjGcY4DyIgIQgnMgNIei4FW-3EIDzCjJMYZiGw0AMMtZhcUe2o9xtippCbtoj5a0KrDgA_Qw9K6zrvd9zPpoMl9nCxt71WTW1T60qrPeZWrk-y7rpiab-2g_jhK4qlu_sG6STZUbH6KdWjXRHG32A_Ryc_08vMsfn27vh1ePuWak6HJRMl6POFDKBBPaUMbZiEIF1BjgTBNtBOW4MGPOFMdcCV6rijFd6bRARQ_Q5Vp33o9aM9bGdUE1ch5sq8Kb9MrK3x1np3LiFxJDci0ITQpnG4XgX3sTO9naqE3TKGfSR0hSUcJLAaxM6OkfdOb74JK_RDHKyhI4ThRZUzr4GIOpv16DQa6ykuusZMpKfmQll2no5KePr5HPcBJA10BMLTcx4fvuf2TfAS3tn34</recordid><startdate>20230801</startdate><enddate>20230801</enddate><creator>Brenner, Eli</creator><creator>Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>C6C</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3611-2843</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20230801</creationdate><title>Continuous use of visual information about the position of the moving hand</title><author>Brenner, Eli ; Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-9745fb50334949ce3454b30803ee054c2ce93516ed54a515a95fa844c8c44c083</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain research</topic><topic>Fingers</topic><topic>Hand</topic><topic>Hand - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Motion Perception - physiology</topic><topic>Movement - physiology</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Research Article</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Brenner, Eli</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</creatorcontrib><collection>Springer Nature OA Free Journals</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Brenner, Eli</au><au>Smeets, Jeroen B. J.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Continuous use of visual information about the position of the moving hand</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><stitle>Exp Brain Res</stitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2023-08-01</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>241</volume><issue>8</issue><spage>2001</spage><epage>2008</epage><pages>2001-2008</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><abstract>People generally look at a target when they want to reach for it. Doing so presumably helps them continuously update their judgments about the target’s position and motion. But not looking at their hand does not prevent people from updating judgments about its position on the basis of visual information, because people do respond to experimental perturbations of visual information about the position of their hand. Here, we study such responses by adding jitter to the movement of a cursor that follows participants’ fingers. We analyse the response to the jitter in a way that reveals how the vigour of the response depends on the moment during the movement at which the change in cursor position occurs. We compare the change in vigour to that for equivalent jitter in the position of the target. We find that participants respond to jitter in the position of a cursor in much the same way as they respond to jitter in the target’s position. The responses are more vigorous late in the movement, when adjustments need to be made within less time, but similarly so for the cursor as for the target. The responses are weaker for the cursor, presumably because of the jitter-free kinaesthetic information about the position of the finger.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>37386195</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-023-06658-x</doi><tpages>8</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3611-2843</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0014-4819 |
ispartof | Experimental brain research, 2023-08, Vol.241 (8), p.2001-2008 |
issn | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_10386923 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings |
subjects | Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedicine Brain research Fingers Hand Hand - physiology Humans Motion Perception - physiology Movement - physiology Neurology Neurosciences Psychomotor Performance - physiology Research Article |
title | Continuous use of visual information about the position of the moving hand |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-07T00%3A06%3A04IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Continuous%20use%20of%20visual%20information%20about%20the%20position%20of%20the%20moving%20hand&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=Brenner,%20Eli&rft.date=2023-08-01&rft.volume=241&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=2001&rft.epage=2008&rft.pages=2001-2008&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-023-06658-x&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2843477051%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2843477051&rft_id=info:pmid/37386195&rfr_iscdi=true |