Signal Detection Analysis of Contingency Assessment: Associative Interference and Nonreinforcement Impact Cue-Outcome Contingency Sensitivity, Whereas Cue Density Affects Bias

In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes 2022-07, Vol.48 (3), p.190-202
Hauptverfasser: Jozefowiez, Jérémie, Urcelay, Gonzalo P., Miller, Ralph R.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 202
container_issue 3
container_start_page 190
container_title Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes
container_volume 48
creator Jozefowiez, Jérémie
Urcelay, Gonzalo P.
Miller, Ralph R.
description In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective contingency is above some threshold. Some researchers have suggested that the mean of the subjective contingency distributions and the threshold are controlled by different variables. The present data provide empirical support for this claim. In three experiments, participants were exposed to rapid streams of trials at the end of which they had to indicate whether a target outcome O1 was more likely following a target cue X. Interfering treatments were incorporated in some streams to impend participants' ability to identify the objective X-O1 contingency: interference trials (X was paired with an irrelevant outcome O2), nonreinforced trials (X was presented alone), plus control trials (an irrelevant cue W was paired with O2). Overall, both interference and nonreinforced trials impaired participants' sensitivity to the contingencies as measured by signal detection theory's d′, but they also enhanced detection of positive contingencies through a cue density effect, with nonreinforced trials being more susceptible to this effect than interference trials. These results are explicable if one assumes interference and nonreinforced trials impact the mean of the associative strength distribution, while the cue density influences the threshold.
doi_str_mv 10.1037/xan0000334
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_10232211</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2693995991</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a505t-b67c78135855d66fa3c14a64228cc5264d459642ef04160c72a8ea23a10880b13</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkktvEzEQx1cIRKvSCx8AWeLCK-D3ermgEB6NVNFDQRwtx5lNXO3aqe2NyKfiK-IlJdBe8MUznp__M2NPVT0m-DXBrH7zw3hcFmP8XnVMGW0mikt-_2ALeVSdpnRVGEKoUAI_rI6YULXCihxXPy_dypsOfYAMNrvg0bS4u-QSCi2aBZ-dX4G3OzRNCVLqwee3ox2sM9ltAc19hthCLBAg45foS_ARnG9DtDDiaN5vjM1oNsDkYsg29HBL-BJ8ckXK5d0r9H1dlEwa4VLTGCiZ27bUltB7Z9Kj6kFrugSnN_tJ9e3Tx6-zs8n5xef5bHo-MQKLPFnI2taKlD6FWErZGmYJN5JTqqwVVPIlF01xocWcSGxrahQYygzBSuEFYSfVu73uZlj0sLSlj2g6vYmuN3Gng3H6dsS7tV6FrSa4PDwlo8LzvcL6zr2z6bkezzBnmEhOtiP77CZbDNcDpKx7lyx0nfEQhqSpbHgja0yagj69g16FIZY_-01Jwigm_D8UaxrRNGPaF3vKxpBShPZQJ8F6nC79d7oK_OTfFzmgf2apAC_3gNkYvUk7a2J2toNkh1imI49imivNNGkw-wWdodrp</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2693995991</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Signal Detection Analysis of Contingency Assessment: Associative Interference and Nonreinforcement Impact Cue-Outcome Contingency Sensitivity, Whereas Cue Density Affects Bias</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>APA PsycARTICLES</source><creator>Jozefowiez, Jérémie ; Urcelay, Gonzalo P. ; Miller, Ralph R.</creator><contributor>Delamater, Andrew R</contributor><creatorcontrib>Jozefowiez, Jérémie ; Urcelay, Gonzalo P. ; Miller, Ralph R. ; Delamater, Andrew R</creatorcontrib><description>In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective contingency is above some threshold. Some researchers have suggested that the mean of the subjective contingency distributions and the threshold are controlled by different variables. The present data provide empirical support for this claim. In three experiments, participants were exposed to rapid streams of trials at the end of which they had to indicate whether a target outcome O1 was more likely following a target cue X. Interfering treatments were incorporated in some streams to impend participants' ability to identify the objective X-O1 contingency: interference trials (X was paired with an irrelevant outcome O2), nonreinforced trials (X was presented alone), plus control trials (an irrelevant cue W was paired with O2). Overall, both interference and nonreinforced trials impaired participants' sensitivity to the contingencies as measured by signal detection theory's d′, but they also enhanced detection of positive contingencies through a cue density effect, with nonreinforced trials being more susceptible to this effect than interference trials. These results are explicable if one assumes interference and nonreinforced trials impact the mean of the associative strength distribution, while the cue density influences the threshold.</description><identifier>ISSN: 2329-8456</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 2329-8464</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/xan0000334</identifier><identifier>PMID: 35878081</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Association Learning ; Associative learning ; Associative Processes ; Bias ; Cognitive Bias ; Cognitive science ; Conditioning, Classical ; Contingency Management ; Cues ; Female ; Human ; Humans ; Interference (Learning) ; Male ; Random variables ; Reinforcement ; Signal Detection (Perception) ; Signal Detection, Psychological ; Streams</subject><ispartof>Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes, 2022-07, Vol.48 (3), p.190-202</ispartof><rights>2022 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>2022, American Psychological Association</rights><rights>Copyright American Psychological Association Jul 2022</rights><rights>Distributed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a505t-b67c78135855d66fa3c14a64228cc5264d459642ef04160c72a8ea23a10880b13</citedby><orcidid>0000-0003-4717-0181 ; 0000-0003-1420-7603</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>230,314,780,784,885,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35878081$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://hal.univ-lille.fr/hal-04301641$$DView record in HAL$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><contributor>Delamater, Andrew R</contributor><creatorcontrib>Jozefowiez, Jérémie</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Urcelay, Gonzalo P.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Miller, Ralph R.</creatorcontrib><title>Signal Detection Analysis of Contingency Assessment: Associative Interference and Nonreinforcement Impact Cue-Outcome Contingency Sensitivity, Whereas Cue Density Affects Bias</title><title>Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes</title><addtitle>J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn</addtitle><description>In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective contingency is above some threshold. Some researchers have suggested that the mean of the subjective contingency distributions and the threshold are controlled by different variables. The present data provide empirical support for this claim. In three experiments, participants were exposed to rapid streams of trials at the end of which they had to indicate whether a target outcome O1 was more likely following a target cue X. Interfering treatments were incorporated in some streams to impend participants' ability to identify the objective X-O1 contingency: interference trials (X was paired with an irrelevant outcome O2), nonreinforced trials (X was presented alone), plus control trials (an irrelevant cue W was paired with O2). Overall, both interference and nonreinforced trials impaired participants' sensitivity to the contingencies as measured by signal detection theory's d′, but they also enhanced detection of positive contingencies through a cue density effect, with nonreinforced trials being more susceptible to this effect than interference trials. These results are explicable if one assumes interference and nonreinforced trials impact the mean of the associative strength distribution, while the cue density influences the threshold.</description><subject>Association Learning</subject><subject>Associative learning</subject><subject>Associative Processes</subject><subject>Bias</subject><subject>Cognitive Bias</subject><subject>Cognitive science</subject><subject>Conditioning, Classical</subject><subject>Contingency Management</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Interference (Learning)</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Random variables</subject><subject>Reinforcement</subject><subject>Signal Detection (Perception)</subject><subject>Signal Detection, Psychological</subject><subject>Streams</subject><issn>2329-8456</issn><issn>2329-8464</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkktvEzEQx1cIRKvSCx8AWeLCK-D3ermgEB6NVNFDQRwtx5lNXO3aqe2NyKfiK-IlJdBe8MUznp__M2NPVT0m-DXBrH7zw3hcFmP8XnVMGW0mikt-_2ALeVSdpnRVGEKoUAI_rI6YULXCihxXPy_dypsOfYAMNrvg0bS4u-QSCi2aBZ-dX4G3OzRNCVLqwee3ox2sM9ltAc19hthCLBAg45foS_ARnG9DtDDiaN5vjM1oNsDkYsg29HBL-BJ8ckXK5d0r9H1dlEwa4VLTGCiZ27bUltB7Z9Kj6kFrugSnN_tJ9e3Tx6-zs8n5xef5bHo-MQKLPFnI2taKlD6FWErZGmYJN5JTqqwVVPIlF01xocWcSGxrahQYygzBSuEFYSfVu73uZlj0sLSlj2g6vYmuN3Gng3H6dsS7tV6FrSa4PDwlo8LzvcL6zr2z6bkezzBnmEhOtiP77CZbDNcDpKx7lyx0nfEQhqSpbHgja0yagj69g16FIZY_-01Jwigm_D8UaxrRNGPaF3vKxpBShPZQJ8F6nC79d7oK_OTfFzmgf2apAC_3gNkYvUk7a2J2toNkh1imI49imivNNGkw-wWdodrp</recordid><startdate>20220701</startdate><enddate>20220701</enddate><creator>Jozefowiez, Jérémie</creator><creator>Urcelay, Gonzalo P.</creator><creator>Miller, Ralph R.</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>1XC</scope><scope>VOOES</scope><scope>5PM</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4717-0181</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1420-7603</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20220701</creationdate><title>Signal Detection Analysis of Contingency Assessment: Associative Interference and Nonreinforcement Impact Cue-Outcome Contingency Sensitivity, Whereas Cue Density Affects Bias</title><author>Jozefowiez, Jérémie ; Urcelay, Gonzalo P. ; Miller, Ralph R.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a505t-b67c78135855d66fa3c14a64228cc5264d459642ef04160c72a8ea23a10880b13</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Association Learning</topic><topic>Associative learning</topic><topic>Associative Processes</topic><topic>Bias</topic><topic>Cognitive Bias</topic><topic>Cognitive science</topic><topic>Conditioning, Classical</topic><topic>Contingency Management</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Interference (Learning)</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Random variables</topic><topic>Reinforcement</topic><topic>Signal Detection (Perception)</topic><topic>Signal Detection, Psychological</topic><topic>Streams</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Jozefowiez, Jérémie</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Urcelay, Gonzalo P.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Miller, Ralph R.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL)</collection><collection>Hyper Article en Ligne (HAL) (Open Access)</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Jozefowiez, Jérémie</au><au>Urcelay, Gonzalo P.</au><au>Miller, Ralph R.</au><au>Delamater, Andrew R</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Signal Detection Analysis of Contingency Assessment: Associative Interference and Nonreinforcement Impact Cue-Outcome Contingency Sensitivity, Whereas Cue Density Affects Bias</atitle><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes</jtitle><addtitle>J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn</addtitle><date>2022-07-01</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>48</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>190</spage><epage>202</epage><pages>190-202</pages><issn>2329-8456</issn><eissn>2329-8464</eissn><abstract>In a signal detection theory approach to associative learning, the perceived (i.e., subjective) contingency between a cue and an outcome is a random variable drawn from a Gaussian distribution. At the end of the sequence, participants report a positive cue-outcome contingency provided the subjective contingency is above some threshold. Some researchers have suggested that the mean of the subjective contingency distributions and the threshold are controlled by different variables. The present data provide empirical support for this claim. In three experiments, participants were exposed to rapid streams of trials at the end of which they had to indicate whether a target outcome O1 was more likely following a target cue X. Interfering treatments were incorporated in some streams to impend participants' ability to identify the objective X-O1 contingency: interference trials (X was paired with an irrelevant outcome O2), nonreinforced trials (X was presented alone), plus control trials (an irrelevant cue W was paired with O2). Overall, both interference and nonreinforced trials impaired participants' sensitivity to the contingencies as measured by signal detection theory's d′, but they also enhanced detection of positive contingencies through a cue density effect, with nonreinforced trials being more susceptible to this effect than interference trials. These results are explicable if one assumes interference and nonreinforced trials impact the mean of the associative strength distribution, while the cue density influences the threshold.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><pmid>35878081</pmid><doi>10.1037/xan0000334</doi><tpages>13</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4717-0181</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1420-7603</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 2329-8456
ispartof Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes, 2022-07, Vol.48 (3), p.190-202
issn 2329-8456
2329-8464
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_10232211
source MEDLINE; APA PsycARTICLES
subjects Association Learning
Associative learning
Associative Processes
Bias
Cognitive Bias
Cognitive science
Conditioning, Classical
Contingency Management
Cues
Female
Human
Humans
Interference (Learning)
Male
Random variables
Reinforcement
Signal Detection (Perception)
Signal Detection, Psychological
Streams
title Signal Detection Analysis of Contingency Assessment: Associative Interference and Nonreinforcement Impact Cue-Outcome Contingency Sensitivity, Whereas Cue Density Affects Bias
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-08T01%3A57%3A16IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Signal%20Detection%20Analysis%20of%20Contingency%20Assessment:%20Associative%20Interference%20and%20Nonreinforcement%20Impact%20Cue-Outcome%20Contingency%20Sensitivity,%20Whereas%20Cue%20Density%20Affects%20Bias&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology.%20Animal%20behavior%20processes&rft.au=Jozefowiez,%20J%C3%A9r%C3%A9mie&rft.date=2022-07-01&rft.volume=48&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=190&rft.epage=202&rft.pages=190-202&rft.issn=2329-8456&rft.eissn=2329-8464&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/xan0000334&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_pubme%3E2693995991%3C/proquest_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2693995991&rft_id=info:pmid/35878081&rfr_iscdi=true