Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children's reasoning about the natural world

What is the relation between human and nonhuman animals? As adults, we construe this relation flexibly, depending in part on the situation at hand. From a biological perspective, we acknowledge the status of humans as one species among many (as in Western science), but at the same time may adopt oth...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS 2010-06, Vol.107 (22), p.9979-9984
Hauptverfasser: Herrmann, Patricia, Waxman, Sandra R, Medin, Douglas L
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 9984
container_issue 22
container_start_page 9979
container_title Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS
container_volume 107
creator Herrmann, Patricia
Waxman, Sandra R
Medin, Douglas L
description What is the relation between human and nonhuman animals? As adults, we construe this relation flexibly, depending in part on the situation at hand. From a biological perspective, we acknowledge the status of humans as one species among many (as in Western science), but at the same time may adopt other perspectives, including an anthropocentric perspective in which human characteristics are attributed to nonhuman animals (as in fables and popular media). How do these perspectives develop? The predominant view in developmental cognitive science is that young children universally possess only one markedly anthropocentric vantage point, and must undergo fundamental conceptual change, overturning their initially human-centered framework before they can acquire a distinctly biological framework. Evidence from two experiments challenges this view. By developing a task that allows us to test children as young as 3 years of age, we are able to demonstrate that anthropocentrism is not the first developmental step in children's reasoning about the biological world. Although urban 5-year-olds adopt an anthropocentric perspective, replicating previous reports, 3-year-olds show no hint of anthropocentrism. This suggests a previously unexplored model of development: Anthropocentrism is not an initial step in conceptual development, but is instead an acquired perspective, one that emerges between 3 and 5 years of age in children raised in urban environments.
doi_str_mv 10.1073/pnas.1004440107
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstor_pubme</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_pubmed_primary_20479241</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>25681716</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>25681716</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c457t-bd29062a99560f4221a9f11630d36e024f0ec7f3c4721783405e50a742b1ac9b3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNpVkc9vFCEUx4nR2O3q2ZNKvHga-_gxMFyaNI1VkyY9aL0SZobZZTMLIzAa__uy7rrbniDweR8e74vQGwKfCEh2MXmTyg4451AOnqEFAUUqwRU8RwsAKquGU36GzlPaAICqG3iJzihwqSgnC_Tzyud1DFPorM_RpS12CfuQcV5bPLiYMk7ZTth53K3d2EfrPyYcrUnBO7_Cpg3zHvYmz9GM-E-IY_8KvRjMmOzrw7pE9zeff1x_rW7vvny7vrqtOl7LXLU9VSCoUaoWMHBKiVEDIYJBz4QFygewnRxYxyUlsmEcaluDkZy2xHSqZUt0ufdOc7u1_b9PmFFP0W1N_KuDcfrpjXdrvQq_NW0UcEGK4MNBEMOv2aasN2GOvvSsmag5MFnGvEQXe6iLIaVoh-MDBPQuB73LQZ9yKBXvHvd15P8PvgDvD8Cu8qSTmlKtlFSFeLsnNimHeDLUoiGyjOhoGEzQZlWy0_ffKRAGpKlBCMkeANY1oWw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Open Access Repository</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>365403707</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children's reasoning about the natural world</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><source>PubMed Central</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><source>Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry</source><creator>Herrmann, Patricia ; Waxman, Sandra R ; Medin, Douglas L</creator><creatorcontrib>Herrmann, Patricia ; Waxman, Sandra R ; Medin, Douglas L</creatorcontrib><description>What is the relation between human and nonhuman animals? As adults, we construe this relation flexibly, depending in part on the situation at hand. From a biological perspective, we acknowledge the status of humans as one species among many (as in Western science), but at the same time may adopt other perspectives, including an anthropocentric perspective in which human characteristics are attributed to nonhuman animals (as in fables and popular media). How do these perspectives develop? The predominant view in developmental cognitive science is that young children universally possess only one markedly anthropocentric vantage point, and must undergo fundamental conceptual change, overturning their initially human-centered framework before they can acquire a distinctly biological framework. Evidence from two experiments challenges this view. By developing a task that allows us to test children as young as 3 years of age, we are able to demonstrate that anthropocentrism is not the first developmental step in children's reasoning about the biological world. Although urban 5-year-olds adopt an anthropocentric perspective, replicating previous reports, 3-year-olds show no hint of anthropocentrism. This suggests a previously unexplored model of development: Anthropocentrism is not an initial step in conceptual development, but is instead an acquired perspective, one that emerges between 3 and 5 years of age in children raised in urban environments.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0027-8424</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1091-6490</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1004440107</identifier><identifier>PMID: 20479241</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: National Academy of Sciences</publisher><subject>Animals ; Anthropocentrism ; Child Development ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Children &amp; youth ; Cognition ; Cognition &amp; reasoning ; Concept Formation ; Dogs ; Experiments ; Female ; Humans ; Inductive reasoning ; Learning ; Male ; Mammals ; Models, Psychological ; Psychological Theory ; Puppets ; Reasoning ; School age children ; Social Environment ; Social Sciences ; Thinking ; Urban Population</subject><ispartof>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 2010-06, Vol.107 (22), p.9979-9984</ispartof><rights>Copyright National Academy of Sciences Jun 1, 2010</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c457t-bd29062a99560f4221a9f11630d36e024f0ec7f3c4721783405e50a742b1ac9b3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c457t-bd29062a99560f4221a9f11630d36e024f0ec7f3c4721783405e50a742b1ac9b3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Uhttp://www.pnas.org/content/107/22.cover.gif</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/25681716$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/25681716$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>230,314,727,780,784,803,885,27924,27925,53791,53793,58017,58250</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20479241$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Herrmann, Patricia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Waxman, Sandra R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Medin, Douglas L</creatorcontrib><title>Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children's reasoning about the natural world</title><title>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS</title><addtitle>Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A</addtitle><description>What is the relation between human and nonhuman animals? As adults, we construe this relation flexibly, depending in part on the situation at hand. From a biological perspective, we acknowledge the status of humans as one species among many (as in Western science), but at the same time may adopt other perspectives, including an anthropocentric perspective in which human characteristics are attributed to nonhuman animals (as in fables and popular media). How do these perspectives develop? The predominant view in developmental cognitive science is that young children universally possess only one markedly anthropocentric vantage point, and must undergo fundamental conceptual change, overturning their initially human-centered framework before they can acquire a distinctly biological framework. Evidence from two experiments challenges this view. By developing a task that allows us to test children as young as 3 years of age, we are able to demonstrate that anthropocentrism is not the first developmental step in children's reasoning about the biological world. Although urban 5-year-olds adopt an anthropocentric perspective, replicating previous reports, 3-year-olds show no hint of anthropocentrism. This suggests a previously unexplored model of development: Anthropocentrism is not an initial step in conceptual development, but is instead an acquired perspective, one that emerges between 3 and 5 years of age in children raised in urban environments.</description><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Anthropocentrism</subject><subject>Child Development</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Children &amp; youth</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Concept Formation</subject><subject>Dogs</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Inductive reasoning</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mammals</subject><subject>Models, Psychological</subject><subject>Psychological Theory</subject><subject>Puppets</subject><subject>Reasoning</subject><subject>School age children</subject><subject>Social Environment</subject><subject>Social Sciences</subject><subject>Thinking</subject><subject>Urban Population</subject><issn>0027-8424</issn><issn>1091-6490</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2010</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNpVkc9vFCEUx4nR2O3q2ZNKvHga-_gxMFyaNI1VkyY9aL0SZobZZTMLIzAa__uy7rrbniDweR8e74vQGwKfCEh2MXmTyg4451AOnqEFAUUqwRU8RwsAKquGU36GzlPaAICqG3iJzihwqSgnC_Tzyud1DFPorM_RpS12CfuQcV5bPLiYMk7ZTth53K3d2EfrPyYcrUnBO7_Cpg3zHvYmz9GM-E-IY_8KvRjMmOzrw7pE9zeff1x_rW7vvny7vrqtOl7LXLU9VSCoUaoWMHBKiVEDIYJBz4QFygewnRxYxyUlsmEcaluDkZy2xHSqZUt0ufdOc7u1_b9PmFFP0W1N_KuDcfrpjXdrvQq_NW0UcEGK4MNBEMOv2aasN2GOvvSsmag5MFnGvEQXe6iLIaVoh-MDBPQuB73LQZ9yKBXvHvd15P8PvgDvD8Cu8qSTmlKtlFSFeLsnNimHeDLUoiGyjOhoGEzQZlWy0_ffKRAGpKlBCMkeANY1oWw</recordid><startdate>20100601</startdate><enddate>20100601</enddate><creator>Herrmann, Patricia</creator><creator>Waxman, Sandra R</creator><creator>Medin, Douglas L</creator><general>National Academy of Sciences</general><general>National Acad Sciences</general><scope>FBQ</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QL</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7SS</scope><scope>7T5</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7TO</scope><scope>7U9</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>H94</scope><scope>M7N</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>5PM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20100601</creationdate><title>Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children's reasoning about the natural world</title><author>Herrmann, Patricia ; Waxman, Sandra R ; Medin, Douglas L</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c457t-bd29062a99560f4221a9f11630d36e024f0ec7f3c4721783405e50a742b1ac9b3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2010</creationdate><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Anthropocentrism</topic><topic>Child Development</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Children &amp; youth</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Concept Formation</topic><topic>Dogs</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Inductive reasoning</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mammals</topic><topic>Models, Psychological</topic><topic>Psychological Theory</topic><topic>Puppets</topic><topic>Reasoning</topic><topic>School age children</topic><topic>Social Environment</topic><topic>Social Sciences</topic><topic>Thinking</topic><topic>Urban Population</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Herrmann, Patricia</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Waxman, Sandra R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Medin, Douglas L</creatorcontrib><collection>AGRIS</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Bacteriology Abstracts (Microbiology B)</collection><collection>Calcium &amp; Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Entomology Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Immunology Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Oncogenes and Growth Factors Abstracts</collection><collection>Virology and AIDS Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>AIDS and Cancer Research Abstracts</collection><collection>Algology Mycology and Protozoology Abstracts (Microbiology C)</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)</collection><jtitle>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Herrmann, Patricia</au><au>Waxman, Sandra R</au><au>Medin, Douglas L</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children's reasoning about the natural world</atitle><jtitle>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS</jtitle><addtitle>Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A</addtitle><date>2010-06-01</date><risdate>2010</risdate><volume>107</volume><issue>22</issue><spage>9979</spage><epage>9984</epage><pages>9979-9984</pages><issn>0027-8424</issn><eissn>1091-6490</eissn><abstract>What is the relation between human and nonhuman animals? As adults, we construe this relation flexibly, depending in part on the situation at hand. From a biological perspective, we acknowledge the status of humans as one species among many (as in Western science), but at the same time may adopt other perspectives, including an anthropocentric perspective in which human characteristics are attributed to nonhuman animals (as in fables and popular media). How do these perspectives develop? The predominant view in developmental cognitive science is that young children universally possess only one markedly anthropocentric vantage point, and must undergo fundamental conceptual change, overturning their initially human-centered framework before they can acquire a distinctly biological framework. Evidence from two experiments challenges this view. By developing a task that allows us to test children as young as 3 years of age, we are able to demonstrate that anthropocentrism is not the first developmental step in children's reasoning about the biological world. Although urban 5-year-olds adopt an anthropocentric perspective, replicating previous reports, 3-year-olds show no hint of anthropocentrism. This suggests a previously unexplored model of development: Anthropocentrism is not an initial step in conceptual development, but is instead an acquired perspective, one that emerges between 3 and 5 years of age in children raised in urban environments.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>National Academy of Sciences</pub><pmid>20479241</pmid><doi>10.1073/pnas.1004440107</doi><tpages>6</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0027-8424
ispartof Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 2010-06, Vol.107 (22), p.9979-9984
issn 0027-8424
1091-6490
language eng
recordid cdi_pubmed_primary_20479241
source MEDLINE; JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing; PubMed Central; Alma/SFX Local Collection; Free Full-Text Journals in Chemistry
subjects Animals
Anthropocentrism
Child Development
Child, Preschool
Children
Children & youth
Cognition
Cognition & reasoning
Concept Formation
Dogs
Experiments
Female
Humans
Inductive reasoning
Learning
Male
Mammals
Models, Psychological
Psychological Theory
Puppets
Reasoning
School age children
Social Environment
Social Sciences
Thinking
Urban Population
title Anthropocentrism is not the first step in children's reasoning about the natural world
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-29T14%3A09%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_pubme&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Anthropocentrism%20is%20not%20the%20first%20step%20in%20children's%20reasoning%20about%20the%20natural%20world&rft.jtitle=Proceedings%20of%20the%20National%20Academy%20of%20Sciences%20-%20PNAS&rft.au=Herrmann,%20Patricia&rft.date=2010-06-01&rft.volume=107&rft.issue=22&rft.spage=9979&rft.epage=9984&rft.pages=9979-9984&rft.issn=0027-8424&rft.eissn=1091-6490&rft_id=info:doi/10.1073/pnas.1004440107&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_pubme%3E25681716%3C/jstor_pubme%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=365403707&rft_id=info:pmid/20479241&rft_jstor_id=25681716&rfr_iscdi=true