Deceived and Distorted: Game Outcome Retrospectively Determines the Reported Time of Action
Previous work suggested the association between intentionality and the reported time of action was exclusive, with intentionality as the primary facilitator to the mental time compression between the reported time of action and its effect (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). In three experiments...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance 2011-10, Vol.37 (5), p.1458-1469 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 1469 |
---|---|
container_issue | 5 |
container_start_page | 1458 |
container_title | Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance |
container_volume | 37 |
creator | Isham, Eve A Banks, William P Ekstrom, Arne D Stern, Jessica A |
description | Previous work suggested the association between intentionality and the reported time of action was exclusive, with intentionality as the primary facilitator to the mental time compression between the reported time of action and its effect (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). In three experiments, we examined whether mental time compression could also be observed in an unintended action. Participants performed an externally cued key press task that elicited one of two possible tones. The reported time of action shifted closer to the tone when the tone was used to indicate the winner of a race (Exp.2) compared to when the tone was meaningless and did not indicate winning (Exp.1). This suggests that reported time of an unintended action could shift toward the effect in some contexts. Furthermore, the results from Exp.2 and Exp.3 (tones were substituted with verbal feedback) showed that a presumed winning action was judged to occur earlier whereas a presumed losing action was judged to be later. These findings therefore support the view that the reported time of action is reconstructed from known temporal information rather than determined by intentionality. (Contains 5 tables and 6 figures.) |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/a0023111 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_919969561</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ945656</ericid><sourcerecordid>902369501</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c451t-9727f97a06409401eae962d81439baf5ce7fcdf3929d3419d560c652524e7d733</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0c9rFTEQB_Agin1WwT9AZBGkXlYz-bnjrfS1VSkUpJ48LGkyi1v2xzPJCv3vzfO9VvBgc5nDfDIk82XsJfD3wKX94DgXEgAesRWgxBqEtY_ZinM0NWghD9izlG54OdDop-xAgC49pVbs-5o89b8oVG4K1bpPeY6Zwsfq3I1UXS7Zz6V-pRzntCGfCx1uqzVlimM_Uaryj2178-dWddUXPHfVcYHz9Jw96dyQ6MW-HrJvZ6dXJ5_qi8vzzyfHF7VXGnKNVtgOreNGlTdxIEdoRGhASbx2nfZkOx86iQKDVIBBG-6NFloossFKeciOdnM3cf65UMrt2CdPw-AmmpfUIiAa1AYelmWNRfKHZYOqAW1QFPnmH3kzL3EqHy4IjUZeIvofMsI20DRb9G6HfFl2itS1m9iPLt62wNttzu1dzoW-3s9brkcK9_Au2ALe7oFL3g1ddJPv01-nNIKGrXu1cxR7f98-_YJKG23kb6ZCtKM</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>862781880</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Deceived and Distorted: Game Outcome Retrospectively Determines the Reported Time of Action</title><source>APA PsycARTICLES</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>MEDLINE</source><creator>Isham, Eve A ; Banks, William P ; Ekstrom, Arne D ; Stern, Jessica A</creator><creatorcontrib>Isham, Eve A ; Banks, William P ; Ekstrom, Arne D ; Stern, Jessica A</creatorcontrib><description>Previous work suggested the association between intentionality and the reported time of action was exclusive, with intentionality as the primary facilitator to the mental time compression between the reported time of action and its effect (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). In three experiments, we examined whether mental time compression could also be observed in an unintended action. Participants performed an externally cued key press task that elicited one of two possible tones. The reported time of action shifted closer to the tone when the tone was used to indicate the winner of a race (Exp.2) compared to when the tone was meaningless and did not indicate winning (Exp.1). This suggests that reported time of an unintended action could shift toward the effect in some contexts. Furthermore, the results from Exp.2 and Exp.3 (tones were substituted with verbal feedback) showed that a presumed winning action was judged to occur earlier whereas a presumed losing action was judged to be later. These findings therefore support the view that the reported time of action is reconstructed from known temporal information rather than determined by intentionality. (Contains 5 tables and 6 figures.)</description><identifier>ISSN: 0096-1523</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-1277</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/a0023111</identifier><identifier>PMID: 21500944</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JPHPDH</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, DC: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Acoustic Stimulation ; Activity levels. Psychomotricity ; Adolescent ; Adult ; Agency ; Attention ; Attitudes ; Auditory Perception ; Auditory Stimuli ; Biological and medical sciences ; California ; Cognition & reasoning ; Context Effect ; Contextual Associations ; Cues ; Experimental psychology ; Facilitators ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Games ; Games, Experimental ; Human ; Humans ; Intention ; Judgment ; Male ; Pattern Recognition, Visual ; Perceptions ; Perceptual Distortion ; Pitch Perception ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Psychomotor activities ; Psychomotor Performance ; Speech Perception ; Task performance ; Time Perception ; Time Perspective ; Undergraduate Students ; Winners ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 2011-10, Vol.37 (5), p.1458-1469</ispartof><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>2011, American Psychological Association</rights><rights>Copyright American Psychological Association Oct 2011</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c451t-9727f97a06409401eae962d81439baf5ce7fcdf3929d3419d560c652524e7d733</citedby><orcidid>0000-0002-6812-2368</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902,30976,30977</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ945656$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=24591514$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21500944$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Isham, Eve A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Banks, William P</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ekstrom, Arne D</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Stern, Jessica A</creatorcontrib><title>Deceived and Distorted: Game Outcome Retrospectively Determines the Reported Time of Action</title><title>Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance</title><addtitle>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</addtitle><description>Previous work suggested the association between intentionality and the reported time of action was exclusive, with intentionality as the primary facilitator to the mental time compression between the reported time of action and its effect (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). In three experiments, we examined whether mental time compression could also be observed in an unintended action. Participants performed an externally cued key press task that elicited one of two possible tones. The reported time of action shifted closer to the tone when the tone was used to indicate the winner of a race (Exp.2) compared to when the tone was meaningless and did not indicate winning (Exp.1). This suggests that reported time of an unintended action could shift toward the effect in some contexts. Furthermore, the results from Exp.2 and Exp.3 (tones were substituted with verbal feedback) showed that a presumed winning action was judged to occur earlier whereas a presumed losing action was judged to be later. These findings therefore support the view that the reported time of action is reconstructed from known temporal information rather than determined by intentionality. (Contains 5 tables and 6 figures.)</description><subject>Acoustic Stimulation</subject><subject>Activity levels. Psychomotricity</subject><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Agency</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Attitudes</subject><subject>Auditory Perception</subject><subject>Auditory Stimuli</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>California</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Context Effect</subject><subject>Contextual Associations</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Experimental psychology</subject><subject>Facilitators</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Games</subject><subject>Games, Experimental</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Intention</subject><subject>Judgment</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Pattern Recognition, Visual</subject><subject>Perceptions</subject><subject>Perceptual Distortion</subject><subject>Pitch Perception</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Psychomotor activities</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance</subject><subject>Speech Perception</subject><subject>Task performance</subject><subject>Time Perception</subject><subject>Time Perspective</subject><subject>Undergraduate Students</subject><subject>Winners</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0096-1523</issn><issn>1939-1277</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2011</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0c9rFTEQB_Agin1WwT9AZBGkXlYz-bnjrfS1VSkUpJ48LGkyi1v2xzPJCv3vzfO9VvBgc5nDfDIk82XsJfD3wKX94DgXEgAesRWgxBqEtY_ZinM0NWghD9izlG54OdDop-xAgC49pVbs-5o89b8oVG4K1bpPeY6Zwsfq3I1UXS7Zz6V-pRzntCGfCx1uqzVlimM_Uaryj2178-dWddUXPHfVcYHz9Jw96dyQ6MW-HrJvZ6dXJ5_qi8vzzyfHF7VXGnKNVtgOreNGlTdxIEdoRGhASbx2nfZkOx86iQKDVIBBG-6NFloossFKeciOdnM3cf65UMrt2CdPw-AmmpfUIiAa1AYelmWNRfKHZYOqAW1QFPnmH3kzL3EqHy4IjUZeIvofMsI20DRb9G6HfFl2itS1m9iPLt62wNttzu1dzoW-3s9brkcK9_Au2ALe7oFL3g1ddJPv01-nNIKGrXu1cxR7f98-_YJKG23kb6ZCtKM</recordid><startdate>20111001</startdate><enddate>20111001</enddate><creator>Isham, Eve A</creator><creator>Banks, William P</creator><creator>Ekstrom, Arne D</creator><creator>Stern, Jessica A</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6812-2368</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20111001</creationdate><title>Deceived and Distorted: Game Outcome Retrospectively Determines the Reported Time of Action</title><author>Isham, Eve A ; Banks, William P ; Ekstrom, Arne D ; Stern, Jessica A</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c451t-9727f97a06409401eae962d81439baf5ce7fcdf3929d3419d560c652524e7d733</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2011</creationdate><topic>Acoustic Stimulation</topic><topic>Activity levels. Psychomotricity</topic><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Agency</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Attitudes</topic><topic>Auditory Perception</topic><topic>Auditory Stimuli</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>California</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Context Effect</topic><topic>Contextual Associations</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Experimental psychology</topic><topic>Facilitators</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Games</topic><topic>Games, Experimental</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Intention</topic><topic>Judgment</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Pattern Recognition, Visual</topic><topic>Perceptions</topic><topic>Perceptual Distortion</topic><topic>Pitch Perception</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Psychomotor activities</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance</topic><topic>Speech Perception</topic><topic>Task performance</topic><topic>Time Perception</topic><topic>Time Perspective</topic><topic>Undergraduate Students</topic><topic>Winners</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Isham, Eve A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Banks, William P</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ekstrom, Arne D</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Stern, Jessica A</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Isham, Eve A</au><au>Banks, William P</au><au>Ekstrom, Arne D</au><au>Stern, Jessica A</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ945656</ericid><atitle>Deceived and Distorted: Game Outcome Retrospectively Determines the Reported Time of Action</atitle><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance</jtitle><addtitle>J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform</addtitle><date>2011-10-01</date><risdate>2011</risdate><volume>37</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>1458</spage><epage>1469</epage><pages>1458-1469</pages><issn>0096-1523</issn><eissn>1939-1277</eissn><coden>JPHPDH</coden><abstract>Previous work suggested the association between intentionality and the reported time of action was exclusive, with intentionality as the primary facilitator to the mental time compression between the reported time of action and its effect (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). In three experiments, we examined whether mental time compression could also be observed in an unintended action. Participants performed an externally cued key press task that elicited one of two possible tones. The reported time of action shifted closer to the tone when the tone was used to indicate the winner of a race (Exp.2) compared to when the tone was meaningless and did not indicate winning (Exp.1). This suggests that reported time of an unintended action could shift toward the effect in some contexts. Furthermore, the results from Exp.2 and Exp.3 (tones were substituted with verbal feedback) showed that a presumed winning action was judged to occur earlier whereas a presumed losing action was judged to be later. These findings therefore support the view that the reported time of action is reconstructed from known temporal information rather than determined by intentionality. (Contains 5 tables and 6 figures.)</abstract><cop>Washington, DC</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><pmid>21500944</pmid><doi>10.1037/a0023111</doi><tpages>12</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6812-2368</orcidid></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0096-1523 |
ispartof | Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 2011-10, Vol.37 (5), p.1458-1469 |
issn | 0096-1523 1939-1277 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_919969561 |
source | APA PsycARTICLES; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); MEDLINE |
subjects | Acoustic Stimulation Activity levels. Psychomotricity Adolescent Adult Agency Attention Attitudes Auditory Perception Auditory Stimuli Biological and medical sciences California Cognition & reasoning Context Effect Contextual Associations Cues Experimental psychology Facilitators Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Games Games, Experimental Human Humans Intention Judgment Male Pattern Recognition, Visual Perceptions Perceptual Distortion Pitch Perception Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Psychomotor activities Psychomotor Performance Speech Perception Task performance Time Perception Time Perspective Undergraduate Students Winners Young Adult |
title | Deceived and Distorted: Game Outcome Retrospectively Determines the Reported Time of Action |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-02T06%3A48%3A24IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Deceived%20and%20Distorted:%20Game%20Outcome%20Retrospectively%20Determines%20the%20Reported%20Time%20of%20Action&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology.%20Human%20perception%20and%20performance&rft.au=Isham,%20Eve%20A&rft.date=2011-10-01&rft.volume=37&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=1458&rft.epage=1469&rft.pages=1458-1469&rft.issn=0096-1523&rft.eissn=1939-1277&rft.coden=JPHPDH&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/a0023111&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E902369501%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=862781880&rft_id=info:pmid/21500944&rft_ericid=EJ945656&rfr_iscdi=true |