A motion aftereffect from visual imagery of motion

Mental imagery is thought to share properties with perception. To what extent does the process of imagining a scene share neural circuits and computational mechanisms with actually perceiving the same scene? Here, we investigated whether mental imagery of motion in a particular direction recruits ne...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Cognition 2010-02, Vol.114 (2), p.276-284
Hauptverfasser: Winawer, Jonathan, Huk, Alexander C., Boroditsky, Lera
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 284
container_issue 2
container_start_page 276
container_title Cognition
container_volume 114
creator Winawer, Jonathan
Huk, Alexander C.
Boroditsky, Lera
description Mental imagery is thought to share properties with perception. To what extent does the process of imagining a scene share neural circuits and computational mechanisms with actually perceiving the same scene? Here, we investigated whether mental imagery of motion in a particular direction recruits neural circuits tuned to the same direction of perceptual motion. To address this question we made use of a visual illusion, the motion aftereffect. We found that following prolonged imagery of motion in one direction, people are more likely to perceive real motion test probes as moving in the direction opposite to the direction of motion imagery. The transfer of adaptation from imagined to perceived motion provides evidence that motion imagery and motion perception recruit shared direction-selective neural circuitry. Even in the absence of any visual stimuli, people can selectively recruit specific low-level sensory neurons through mental imagery.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.010
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_877570876</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ869937</ericid><els_id>S0010027709002236</els_id><sourcerecordid>37281136</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c550t-afae6fd58731862edf5f7c2c354db663266bd9ce2392cb9e1a869db256ec665d3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkUtr3DAUhUVpaSZp_0FpvWm68lQP67UcQpK2BLpJ1kKWroIG20olTyD_vjJjprsGLgh0vvvgHIS-ELwlmIjv-61Lj1OcY5q2FGO9XYrgN2hDlGStVEy9RRtcv1pMpTxD56XsMcYdleo9OiNacUY7sUF014xpGdPYMEOGEMDNTchpbJ5jOdihiaN9hPzSpLCSH9C7YIcCH9f3Aj3cXN9f_Wjvft_-vNrdtY5zPLc2WBDB83oPUYKCDzxIRx3jne-FYFSI3msHlGnqeg3EKqF9T7kAJwT37AJ9O859yunPAcpsxlgcDIOdIB2KUVJyiZUUr5KSsXqB1At5-V-SSaoIYQsoj6DLqZTqi3nK1Yn8Ygg2SwRmb04RmCUCsxTBtfPzuuLQj-D_9a2eV-DrCtji7BCynVwsJ45SJrtOk8p9OnKQozvJ17-qTZrJKu9WuWbwHCGb4iJMDnzMNUPjU3z11r_1g7E2</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>37281136</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>A motion aftereffect from visual imagery of motion</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Winawer, Jonathan ; Huk, Alexander C. ; Boroditsky, Lera</creator><creatorcontrib>Winawer, Jonathan ; Huk, Alexander C. ; Boroditsky, Lera</creatorcontrib><description>Mental imagery is thought to share properties with perception. To what extent does the process of imagining a scene share neural circuits and computational mechanisms with actually perceiving the same scene? Here, we investigated whether mental imagery of motion in a particular direction recruits neural circuits tuned to the same direction of perceptual motion. To address this question we made use of a visual illusion, the motion aftereffect. We found that following prolonged imagery of motion in one direction, people are more likely to perceive real motion test probes as moving in the direction opposite to the direction of motion imagery. The transfer of adaptation from imagined to perceived motion provides evidence that motion imagery and motion perception recruit shared direction-selective neural circuitry. Even in the absence of any visual stimuli, people can selectively recruit specific low-level sensory neurons through mental imagery.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0010-0277</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-7838</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.010</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19853246</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CGTNAU</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Adaptation, Psychological - physiology ; Algorithms ; Biological and medical sciences ; Cognitive Processes ; Cues ; Discrimination (Psychology) - physiology ; Experiments ; Female ; Figural Aftereffect - physiology ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Illusion ; Imagery ; Imagination - physiology ; Learning Experience ; Logistic Models ; Male ; Mind ; Motion ; Music Education ; Neurological Organization ; Neuroscience ; Perception ; Photic Stimulation ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Tests ; Vision ; Visual perception ; Visual Stimuli ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Cognition, 2010-02, Vol.114 (2), p.276-284</ispartof><rights>2009 Elsevier B.V.</rights><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c550t-afae6fd58731862edf5f7c2c354db663266bd9ce2392cb9e1a869db256ec665d3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.010$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3550,27924,27925,45995</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ869937$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=22374491$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19853246$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Winawer, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huk, Alexander C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Boroditsky, Lera</creatorcontrib><title>A motion aftereffect from visual imagery of motion</title><title>Cognition</title><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><description>Mental imagery is thought to share properties with perception. To what extent does the process of imagining a scene share neural circuits and computational mechanisms with actually perceiving the same scene? Here, we investigated whether mental imagery of motion in a particular direction recruits neural circuits tuned to the same direction of perceptual motion. To address this question we made use of a visual illusion, the motion aftereffect. We found that following prolonged imagery of motion in one direction, people are more likely to perceive real motion test probes as moving in the direction opposite to the direction of motion imagery. The transfer of adaptation from imagined to perceived motion provides evidence that motion imagery and motion perception recruit shared direction-selective neural circuitry. Even in the absence of any visual stimuli, people can selectively recruit specific low-level sensory neurons through mental imagery.</description><subject>Adaptation, Psychological - physiology</subject><subject>Algorithms</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cognitive Processes</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Discrimination (Psychology) - physiology</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Figural Aftereffect - physiology</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Illusion</subject><subject>Imagery</subject><subject>Imagination - physiology</subject><subject>Learning Experience</subject><subject>Logistic Models</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mind</subject><subject>Motion</subject><subject>Music Education</subject><subject>Neurological Organization</subject><subject>Neuroscience</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Photic Stimulation</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Tests</subject><subject>Vision</subject><subject>Visual perception</subject><subject>Visual Stimuli</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0010-0277</issn><issn>1873-7838</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2010</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkUtr3DAUhUVpaSZp_0FpvWm68lQP67UcQpK2BLpJ1kKWroIG20olTyD_vjJjprsGLgh0vvvgHIS-ELwlmIjv-61Lj1OcY5q2FGO9XYrgN2hDlGStVEy9RRtcv1pMpTxD56XsMcYdleo9OiNacUY7sUF014xpGdPYMEOGEMDNTchpbJ5jOdihiaN9hPzSpLCSH9C7YIcCH9f3Aj3cXN9f_Wjvft_-vNrdtY5zPLc2WBDB83oPUYKCDzxIRx3jne-FYFSI3msHlGnqeg3EKqF9T7kAJwT37AJ9O859yunPAcpsxlgcDIOdIB2KUVJyiZUUr5KSsXqB1At5-V-SSaoIYQsoj6DLqZTqi3nK1Yn8Ygg2SwRmb04RmCUCsxTBtfPzuuLQj-D_9a2eV-DrCtji7BCynVwsJ45SJrtOk8p9OnKQozvJ17-qTZrJKu9WuWbwHCGb4iJMDnzMNUPjU3z11r_1g7E2</recordid><startdate>20100201</startdate><enddate>20100201</enddate><creator>Winawer, Jonathan</creator><creator>Huk, Alexander C.</creator><creator>Boroditsky, Lera</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7TK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20100201</creationdate><title>A motion aftereffect from visual imagery of motion</title><author>Winawer, Jonathan ; Huk, Alexander C. ; Boroditsky, Lera</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c550t-afae6fd58731862edf5f7c2c354db663266bd9ce2392cb9e1a869db256ec665d3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2010</creationdate><topic>Adaptation, Psychological - physiology</topic><topic>Algorithms</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cognitive Processes</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Discrimination (Psychology) - physiology</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Figural Aftereffect - physiology</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Illusion</topic><topic>Imagery</topic><topic>Imagination - physiology</topic><topic>Learning Experience</topic><topic>Logistic Models</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mind</topic><topic>Motion</topic><topic>Music Education</topic><topic>Neurological Organization</topic><topic>Neuroscience</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Photic Stimulation</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Tests</topic><topic>Vision</topic><topic>Visual perception</topic><topic>Visual Stimuli</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Winawer, Jonathan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Huk, Alexander C.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Boroditsky, Lera</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Winawer, Jonathan</au><au>Huk, Alexander C.</au><au>Boroditsky, Lera</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ869937</ericid><atitle>A motion aftereffect from visual imagery of motion</atitle><jtitle>Cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Cognition</addtitle><date>2010-02-01</date><risdate>2010</risdate><volume>114</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>276</spage><epage>284</epage><pages>276-284</pages><issn>0010-0277</issn><eissn>1873-7838</eissn><coden>CGTNAU</coden><abstract>Mental imagery is thought to share properties with perception. To what extent does the process of imagining a scene share neural circuits and computational mechanisms with actually perceiving the same scene? Here, we investigated whether mental imagery of motion in a particular direction recruits neural circuits tuned to the same direction of perceptual motion. To address this question we made use of a visual illusion, the motion aftereffect. We found that following prolonged imagery of motion in one direction, people are more likely to perceive real motion test probes as moving in the direction opposite to the direction of motion imagery. The transfer of adaptation from imagined to perceived motion provides evidence that motion imagery and motion perception recruit shared direction-selective neural circuitry. Even in the absence of any visual stimuli, people can selectively recruit specific low-level sensory neurons through mental imagery.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>19853246</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.010</doi><tpages>9</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0010-0277
ispartof Cognition, 2010-02, Vol.114 (2), p.276-284
issn 0010-0277
1873-7838
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_877570876
source MEDLINE; Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)
subjects Adaptation, Psychological - physiology
Algorithms
Biological and medical sciences
Cognitive Processes
Cues
Discrimination (Psychology) - physiology
Experiments
Female
Figural Aftereffect - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Humans
Illusion
Imagery
Imagination - physiology
Learning Experience
Logistic Models
Male
Mind
Motion
Music Education
Neurological Organization
Neuroscience
Perception
Photic Stimulation
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Tests
Vision
Visual perception
Visual Stimuli
Young Adult
title A motion aftereffect from visual imagery of motion
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-20T21%3A30%3A57IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=A%20motion%20aftereffect%20from%20visual%20imagery%20of%20motion&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Winawer,%20Jonathan&rft.date=2010-02-01&rft.volume=114&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=276&rft.epage=284&rft.pages=276-284&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.eissn=1873-7838&rft.coden=CGTNAU&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.010&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E37281136%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=37281136&rft_id=info:pmid/19853246&rft_ericid=EJ869937&rft_els_id=S0010027709002236&rfr_iscdi=true