Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall
In immediate free recall, words recalled successively tend to come from nearby serial positions. M. J. Kahana (1996) documented this effect and showed that this tendency, which the authors refer to as the lag recency effect, is well described by a variant of the search of associative memory (SAM) mo...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition memory, and cognition, 1999-07, Vol.25 (4), p.923-941 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 941 |
---|---|
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | 923 |
container_title | Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition |
container_volume | 25 |
creator | Howard, Marc W Kahana, Michael J |
description | In immediate free recall, words recalled successively tend
to come from nearby serial positions.
M. J. Kahana (1996)
documented this effect and
showed that this tendency, which the authors refer to as the
lag recency effect,
is well described by a variant
of the search of associative memory (SAM) model (
J. G. W. Raaijmakers & R. M. Shiffrin, 1980
,
1981
). In
2 experiments, participants performed immediate, delayed, and
continuous distractor free recall under conditions designed to
minimize rehearsal. The lag recency effect, previously observed in
immediate free recall, was also observed in delayed and continuous
distractor free recall. Although two-store memory models, such as
SAM, readily account for the end-of-list recency effect in immediate
free recall, and its attenuation in delayed free recall, these
models fail to account for the long-term recency effect. By means of
analytic simulations, the authors show that both the end of list
recency effect and the lag recency effect, across all distractor
conditions, can be explained by a single-store model in which
context, retrieved with each recalled item, serves as a cue for
subsequent recalls. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85697025</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>85697025</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a487t-747cc6ce12d6d07e8e0a7e5b2d065aeb4190c8888a63e2804799842b3906364d3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkVtrGzEQRkVpSBy3fyAPZWlD3tbV_fIYTG4QSOntVWi1s6Ag77rSLsT_vjI2dSgkmRchODPSfAehM4IXBDP1FVOla8UMW1Cx4AtD2Ts0I4aZmlAt3qPZP-AEneb8iLfF9DE6IZgzIzCZocvl0I_wNE4uVr9dCq4JMYybyvVt9QPKPVbfhhzGMPTVVdeBH3MV-uo6AVTfwbsYP6CjzsUMH_fnHP26vvq5vK3vH27ulpf3teNajbXiynvpgdBWtliBBuwUiIa2WAoHDScGe13KSQZUY66M0Zw2zGDJJG_ZHF3s5q7T8GeCPNpVyB5idD0MU7ZaSKMwFW-C0pTVTUlwjj7_Bz4OU-rLElYSzkpCUrwG0QKp8ntdoC8vQUQzozUhlBSK7iifhpwTdHadwsqljSXYbo3arTC7FWapsNwWo6Xp03701KygfdayU1iA8z3gcvHRJdf7kA-cLrFoecDc2tl13niXxuAjZPsUV4fn_gL1d7B8</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614339565</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Howard, Marc W ; Kahana, Michael J</creator><creatorcontrib>Howard, Marc W ; Kahana, Michael J</creatorcontrib><description>In immediate free recall, words recalled successively tend
to come from nearby serial positions.
M. J. Kahana (1996)
documented this effect and
showed that this tendency, which the authors refer to as the
lag recency effect,
is well described by a variant
of the search of associative memory (SAM) model (
J. G. W. Raaijmakers & R. M. Shiffrin, 1980
,
1981
). In
2 experiments, participants performed immediate, delayed, and
continuous distractor free recall under conditions designed to
minimize rehearsal. The lag recency effect, previously observed in
immediate free recall, was also observed in delayed and continuous
distractor free recall. Although two-store memory models, such as
SAM, readily account for the end-of-list recency effect in immediate
free recall, and its attenuation in delayed free recall, these
models fail to account for the long-term recency effect. By means of
analytic simulations, the authors show that both the end of list
recency effect and the lag recency effect, across all distractor
conditions, can be explained by a single-store model in which
context, retrieved with each recalled item, serves as a cue for
subsequent recalls.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0278-7393</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-1285</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10439501</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JEPCEA</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Washington, DC: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Biological and medical sciences ; Contextual Associations ; Cues ; Free Recall ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Human ; Humans ; Learning. Memory ; Long Term Memory ; Memory ; Mental Recall ; Models, Psychological ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Semantics ; Serial Position Effect ; Short Term Memory ; Time Factors ; Vocabulary</subject><ispartof>Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 1999-07, Vol.25 (4), p.923-941</ispartof><rights>1999 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1999 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright American Psychological Association Jul 1999</rights><rights>1999, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27869,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=1869786$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10439501$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Howard, Marc W</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kahana, Michael J</creatorcontrib><title>Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall</title><title>Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition</title><addtitle>J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn</addtitle><description>In immediate free recall, words recalled successively tend
to come from nearby serial positions.
M. J. Kahana (1996)
documented this effect and
showed that this tendency, which the authors refer to as the
lag recency effect,
is well described by a variant
of the search of associative memory (SAM) model (
J. G. W. Raaijmakers & R. M. Shiffrin, 1980
,
1981
). In
2 experiments, participants performed immediate, delayed, and
continuous distractor free recall under conditions designed to
minimize rehearsal. The lag recency effect, previously observed in
immediate free recall, was also observed in delayed and continuous
distractor free recall. Although two-store memory models, such as
SAM, readily account for the end-of-list recency effect in immediate
free recall, and its attenuation in delayed free recall, these
models fail to account for the long-term recency effect. By means of
analytic simulations, the authors show that both the end of list
recency effect and the lag recency effect, across all distractor
conditions, can be explained by a single-store model in which
context, retrieved with each recalled item, serves as a cue for
subsequent recalls.</description><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Contextual Associations</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Free Recall</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Human</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Learning. Memory</subject><subject>Long Term Memory</subject><subject>Memory</subject><subject>Mental Recall</subject><subject>Models, Psychological</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Serial Position Effect</subject><subject>Short Term Memory</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><subject>Vocabulary</subject><issn>0278-7393</issn><issn>1939-1285</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1999</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkVtrGzEQRkVpSBy3fyAPZWlD3tbV_fIYTG4QSOntVWi1s6Ag77rSLsT_vjI2dSgkmRchODPSfAehM4IXBDP1FVOla8UMW1Cx4AtD2Ts0I4aZmlAt3qPZP-AEneb8iLfF9DE6IZgzIzCZocvl0I_wNE4uVr9dCq4JMYybyvVt9QPKPVbfhhzGMPTVVdeBH3MV-uo6AVTfwbsYP6CjzsUMH_fnHP26vvq5vK3vH27ulpf3teNajbXiynvpgdBWtliBBuwUiIa2WAoHDScGe13KSQZUY66M0Zw2zGDJJG_ZHF3s5q7T8GeCPNpVyB5idD0MU7ZaSKMwFW-C0pTVTUlwjj7_Bz4OU-rLElYSzkpCUrwG0QKp8ntdoC8vQUQzozUhlBSK7iifhpwTdHadwsqljSXYbo3arTC7FWapsNwWo6Xp03701KygfdayU1iA8z3gcvHRJdf7kA-cLrFoecDc2tl13niXxuAjZPsUV4fn_gL1d7B8</recordid><startdate>19990701</startdate><enddate>19990701</enddate><creator>Howard, Marc W</creator><creator>Kahana, Michael J</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7WH</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19990701</creationdate><title>Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall</title><author>Howard, Marc W ; Kahana, Michael J</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a487t-747cc6ce12d6d07e8e0a7e5b2d065aeb4190c8888a63e2804799842b3906364d3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1999</creationdate><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Contextual Associations</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Free Recall</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Human</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Learning. Memory</topic><topic>Long Term Memory</topic><topic>Memory</topic><topic>Mental Recall</topic><topic>Models, Psychological</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Serial Position Effect</topic><topic>Short Term Memory</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><topic>Vocabulary</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Howard, Marc W</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kahana, Michael J</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 50</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Access via APA PsycArticles® (ProQuest)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Howard, Marc W</au><au>Kahana, Michael J</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall</atitle><jtitle>Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition</jtitle><addtitle>J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn</addtitle><date>1999-07-01</date><risdate>1999</risdate><volume>25</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>923</spage><epage>941</epage><pages>923-941</pages><issn>0278-7393</issn><eissn>1939-1285</eissn><coden>JEPCEA</coden><abstract>In immediate free recall, words recalled successively tend
to come from nearby serial positions.
M. J. Kahana (1996)
documented this effect and
showed that this tendency, which the authors refer to as the
lag recency effect,
is well described by a variant
of the search of associative memory (SAM) model (
J. G. W. Raaijmakers & R. M. Shiffrin, 1980
,
1981
). In
2 experiments, participants performed immediate, delayed, and
continuous distractor free recall under conditions designed to
minimize rehearsal. The lag recency effect, previously observed in
immediate free recall, was also observed in delayed and continuous
distractor free recall. Although two-store memory models, such as
SAM, readily account for the end-of-list recency effect in immediate
free recall, and its attenuation in delayed free recall, these
models fail to account for the long-term recency effect. By means of
analytic simulations, the authors show that both the end of list
recency effect and the lag recency effect, across all distractor
conditions, can be explained by a single-store model in which
context, retrieved with each recalled item, serves as a cue for
subsequent recalls.</abstract><cop>Washington, DC</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><pmid>10439501</pmid><doi>10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923</doi><tpages>19</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0278-7393 |
ispartof | Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 1999-07, Vol.25 (4), p.923-941 |
issn | 0278-7393 1939-1285 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85697025 |
source | MEDLINE; EBSCOhost APA PsycARTICLES; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Biological and medical sciences Contextual Associations Cues Free Recall Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Human Humans Learning. Memory Long Term Memory Memory Mental Recall Models, Psychological Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Semantics Serial Position Effect Short Term Memory Time Factors Vocabulary |
title | Contextual Variability and Serial Position Effects in Free Recall |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-24T00%3A44%3A16IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Contextual%20Variability%20and%20Serial%20Position%20Effects%20in%20Free%20Recall&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology.%20Learning,%20memory,%20and%20cognition&rft.au=Howard,%20Marc%20W&rft.date=1999-07-01&rft.volume=25&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=923&rft.epage=941&rft.pages=923-941&rft.issn=0278-7393&rft.eissn=1939-1285&rft.coden=JEPCEA&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.923&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E85697025%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614339565&rft_id=info:pmid/10439501&rfr_iscdi=true |