Word-based morphology
This paper examines two contrasting perspectives on morphological analysis, and considers inflectional patterns that bear on the choice between these alternatives. On what is termed an ABSTRACTIVE perspective, surface word forms are regarded as basic morphotactic units of a grammatical system, with...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of linguistics 2006-11, Vol.42 (3), p.531-573 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 573 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 531 |
container_title | Journal of linguistics |
container_volume | 42 |
creator | BLEVINS, JAMES P. |
description | This paper examines two contrasting perspectives on morphological analysis, and considers inflectional patterns that bear on the choice between these alternatives. On what is termed an ABSTRACTIVE perspective, surface word forms are regarded as basic morphotactic units of a grammatical system, with roots, stems and exponents treated as abstractions over a lexicon of word forms. This traditional standpoint is contrasted with the more CONSTRUCTIVE perspective of post-Bloomfieldian models, in which surface word forms are ‘built’ from sub-word units. Part of the interest of this contrast is that it cuts across conventional divisions of morphological models. Thus, realization-based models are morphosyntactically ‘word-based’ in the sense that they regard words as the minimal meaningful units of a grammatical system. Yet morphotactically, these models tend to adopt a constructive ‘root-based’ or ‘stem-based’ perspective. An examination of some form-class patterns in Saami, Estonian and Georgian highlights advantages of an abstractive model, and suggests that these advantages derive from the fact that sets of words often predict other word forms and determine a morphotactic analysis of their parts, whereas sets of sub-word units are of limited predictive value and typically do not provide enough information to recover word forms. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1017/S0022226706004191 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85672519</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S0022226706004191</cupid><jstor_id>4177008</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>4177008</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c581t-70974d1fe45e20ce12087511dfbd7bf7ef411c1f4c95b8f4e64c6760ee1861d43</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkNFrFDEQh4NY8Lz66IPggwj2bduZTTazedSjrcJBKVftY8hmk3PP3cs12QPvv-8ue1RRxMCQh--bHzPD2BuEcwSkixVAPjxJIAEEKnzGZiikyohE_pzNRpyN_AV7mdIGADmocsZe34dYZ5VJrn7Xhbj7HtqwPpyyE2_a5F4d_zn7enV5t_icLW-uvyw-LjNblNhnBIpEjd6JwuVgHeZQUoFY-6qmypPzAtGiF1YVVemFk8JKkuAclhJrwefsbMrdxfCwd6nXXZOsa1uzdWGfdFlIygtU_xW5lFyAygfx_R_iJuzjdlhCo5KcOB9qznCSbAwpRef1LjadiQeNoMdz6r_OOfR8OAabZE3ro9naJv1qLHNORMXgvZ28TepDfOICiQDKAWcTblLvfj5hE39oSZwKLa9vtVrdffp2zxd6jOPHUU1XxaZeu98W-uewj-FDmKg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>196373337</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Word-based morphology</title><source>Cambridge Journals</source><source>JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing</source><creator>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</creator><creatorcontrib>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</creatorcontrib><description>This paper examines two contrasting perspectives on morphological analysis, and considers inflectional patterns that bear on the choice between these alternatives. On what is termed an ABSTRACTIVE perspective, surface word forms are regarded as basic morphotactic units of a grammatical system, with roots, stems and exponents treated as abstractions over a lexicon of word forms. This traditional standpoint is contrasted with the more CONSTRUCTIVE perspective of post-Bloomfieldian models, in which surface word forms are ‘built’ from sub-word units. Part of the interest of this contrast is that it cuts across conventional divisions of morphological models. Thus, realization-based models are morphosyntactically ‘word-based’ in the sense that they regard words as the minimal meaningful units of a grammatical system. Yet morphotactically, these models tend to adopt a constructive ‘root-based’ or ‘stem-based’ perspective. An examination of some form-class patterns in Saami, Estonian and Georgian highlights advantages of an abstractive model, and suggests that these advantages derive from the fact that sets of words often predict other word forms and determine a morphotactic analysis of their parts, whereas sets of sub-word units are of limited predictive value and typically do not provide enough information to recover word forms.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-2267</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-7742</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0022226706004191</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JLINAJ</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>Descriptive studies and applied theories ; Estonian language ; Etymology ; Georgian language ; Grammar ; Grammatical gender ; Inflection (Morphology) ; Language ; Linguistic anthropology ; Linguistic inflection ; Linguistic morphology ; Linguistics ; Meaning ; Modeling ; Morphological analysis ; Morphology ; Morphosyntax ; Nouns ; Sami languages ; Theory ; Verbs ; Vowels ; Words</subject><ispartof>Journal of linguistics, 2006-11, Vol.42 (3), p.531-573</ispartof><rights>2006 Cambridge University Press</rights><rights>Copyright 2006 Cambridge University Press</rights><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright Cambridge University Press, Publishing Division Nov 2006</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c581t-70974d1fe45e20ce12087511dfbd7bf7ef411c1f4c95b8f4e64c6760ee1861d43</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/4177008$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0022226706004191/type/journal_article$$EHTML$$P50$$Gcambridge$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>164,314,780,784,803,27923,27924,55627,58016,58249</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18237775$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</creatorcontrib><title>Word-based morphology</title><title>Journal of linguistics</title><addtitle>J. Ling</addtitle><description>This paper examines two contrasting perspectives on morphological analysis, and considers inflectional patterns that bear on the choice between these alternatives. On what is termed an ABSTRACTIVE perspective, surface word forms are regarded as basic morphotactic units of a grammatical system, with roots, stems and exponents treated as abstractions over a lexicon of word forms. This traditional standpoint is contrasted with the more CONSTRUCTIVE perspective of post-Bloomfieldian models, in which surface word forms are ‘built’ from sub-word units. Part of the interest of this contrast is that it cuts across conventional divisions of morphological models. Thus, realization-based models are morphosyntactically ‘word-based’ in the sense that they regard words as the minimal meaningful units of a grammatical system. Yet morphotactically, these models tend to adopt a constructive ‘root-based’ or ‘stem-based’ perspective. An examination of some form-class patterns in Saami, Estonian and Georgian highlights advantages of an abstractive model, and suggests that these advantages derive from the fact that sets of words often predict other word forms and determine a morphotactic analysis of their parts, whereas sets of sub-word units are of limited predictive value and typically do not provide enough information to recover word forms.</description><subject>Descriptive studies and applied theories</subject><subject>Estonian language</subject><subject>Etymology</subject><subject>Georgian language</subject><subject>Grammar</subject><subject>Grammatical gender</subject><subject>Inflection (Morphology)</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Linguistic anthropology</subject><subject>Linguistic inflection</subject><subject>Linguistic morphology</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Meaning</subject><subject>Modeling</subject><subject>Morphological analysis</subject><subject>Morphology</subject><subject>Morphosyntax</subject><subject>Nouns</subject><subject>Sami languages</subject><subject>Theory</subject><subject>Verbs</subject><subject>Vowels</subject><subject>Words</subject><issn>0022-2267</issn><issn>1469-7742</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2006</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AIMQZ</sourceid><sourceid>AVQMV</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>K50</sourceid><sourceid>M1D</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><sourceid>PAF</sourceid><sourceid>PQLNA</sourceid><sourceid>PROLI</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkNFrFDEQh4NY8Lz66IPggwj2bduZTTazedSjrcJBKVftY8hmk3PP3cs12QPvv-8ue1RRxMCQh--bHzPD2BuEcwSkixVAPjxJIAEEKnzGZiikyohE_pzNRpyN_AV7mdIGADmocsZe34dYZ5VJrn7Xhbj7HtqwPpyyE2_a5F4d_zn7enV5t_icLW-uvyw-LjNblNhnBIpEjd6JwuVgHeZQUoFY-6qmypPzAtGiF1YVVemFk8JKkuAclhJrwefsbMrdxfCwd6nXXZOsa1uzdWGfdFlIygtU_xW5lFyAygfx_R_iJuzjdlhCo5KcOB9qznCSbAwpRef1LjadiQeNoMdz6r_OOfR8OAabZE3ro9naJv1qLHNORMXgvZ28TepDfOICiQDKAWcTblLvfj5hE39oSZwKLa9vtVrdffp2zxd6jOPHUU1XxaZeu98W-uewj-FDmKg</recordid><startdate>20061101</startdate><enddate>20061101</enddate><creator>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AIMQZ</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AVQMV</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>CLO</scope><scope>CPGLG</scope><scope>CRLPW</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>K50</scope><scope>LIQON</scope><scope>M1D</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PAF</scope><scope>PPXUT</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQLNA</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PROLI</scope><scope>Q9U</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20061101</creationdate><title>Word-based morphology</title><author>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c581t-70974d1fe45e20ce12087511dfbd7bf7ef411c1f4c95b8f4e64c6760ee1861d43</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2006</creationdate><topic>Descriptive studies and applied theories</topic><topic>Estonian language</topic><topic>Etymology</topic><topic>Georgian language</topic><topic>Grammar</topic><topic>Grammatical gender</topic><topic>Inflection (Morphology)</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Linguistic anthropology</topic><topic>Linguistic inflection</topic><topic>Linguistic morphology</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Meaning</topic><topic>Modeling</topic><topic>Morphological analysis</topic><topic>Morphology</topic><topic>Morphosyntax</topic><topic>Nouns</topic><topic>Sami languages</topic><topic>Theory</topic><topic>Verbs</topic><topic>Vowels</topic><topic>Words</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>Arts Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>Literature Online Core (LION Core) (legacy)</collection><collection>Linguistics Collection</collection><collection>Linguistics Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>Art, Design & Architecture Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Literature - U.S. Customers Only</collection><collection>Arts & Humanities Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest Learning: Literature</collection><collection>Literature Online Premium (LION Premium) (legacy)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>Literature Online (LION) - US Customers Only</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>Literature Online (LION)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><jtitle>Journal of linguistics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>BLEVINS, JAMES P.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Word-based morphology</atitle><jtitle>Journal of linguistics</jtitle><addtitle>J. Ling</addtitle><date>2006-11-01</date><risdate>2006</risdate><volume>42</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>531</spage><epage>573</epage><pages>531-573</pages><issn>0022-2267</issn><eissn>1469-7742</eissn><coden>JLINAJ</coden><abstract>This paper examines two contrasting perspectives on morphological analysis, and considers inflectional patterns that bear on the choice between these alternatives. On what is termed an ABSTRACTIVE perspective, surface word forms are regarded as basic morphotactic units of a grammatical system, with roots, stems and exponents treated as abstractions over a lexicon of word forms. This traditional standpoint is contrasted with the more CONSTRUCTIVE perspective of post-Bloomfieldian models, in which surface word forms are ‘built’ from sub-word units. Part of the interest of this contrast is that it cuts across conventional divisions of morphological models. Thus, realization-based models are morphosyntactically ‘word-based’ in the sense that they regard words as the minimal meaningful units of a grammatical system. Yet morphotactically, these models tend to adopt a constructive ‘root-based’ or ‘stem-based’ perspective. An examination of some form-class patterns in Saami, Estonian and Georgian highlights advantages of an abstractive model, and suggests that these advantages derive from the fact that sets of words often predict other word forms and determine a morphotactic analysis of their parts, whereas sets of sub-word units are of limited predictive value and typically do not provide enough information to recover word forms.</abstract><cop>Cambridge, UK</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><doi>10.1017/S0022226706004191</doi><tpages>43</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0022-2267 |
ispartof | Journal of linguistics, 2006-11, Vol.42 (3), p.531-573 |
issn | 0022-2267 1469-7742 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85672519 |
source | Cambridge Journals; JSTOR Archive Collection A-Z Listing |
subjects | Descriptive studies and applied theories Estonian language Etymology Georgian language Grammar Grammatical gender Inflection (Morphology) Language Linguistic anthropology Linguistic inflection Linguistic morphology Linguistics Meaning Modeling Morphological analysis Morphology Morphosyntax Nouns Sami languages Theory Verbs Vowels Words |
title | Word-based morphology |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-11T19%3A07%3A24IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Word-based%20morphology&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20linguistics&rft.au=BLEVINS,%20JAMES%20P.&rft.date=2006-11-01&rft.volume=42&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=531&rft.epage=573&rft.pages=531-573&rft.issn=0022-2267&rft.eissn=1469-7742&rft.coden=JLINAJ&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0022226706004191&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E4177008%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=196373337&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_cupid=10_1017_S0022226706004191&rft_jstor_id=4177008&rfr_iscdi=true |