Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders

In recent years there has been a converging trend in the study of pragmatics, grammar, language acquisition and neurolinguistics which sees as secondary epiphenomena a wide range of phenomena which were once regarded as primary. Examples of this trend in pragmatics include relevance theory, which ex...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of pragmatics 1998, Vol.29 (3), p.291-311
1. Verfasser: Perkins, Michael R.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 311
container_issue 3
container_start_page 291
container_title Journal of pragmatics
container_volume 29
creator Perkins, Michael R.
description In recent years there has been a converging trend in the study of pragmatics, grammar, language acquisition and neurolinguistics which sees as secondary epiphenomena a wide range of phenomena which were once regarded as primary. Examples of this trend in pragmatics include relevance theory, which explains speech acts and implicatures in terms of a more fundamental principle of relevance, and conversation analysis, which sees notions such as ‘topic’ as by-products of conversational coherence. In this paper I adopt a similarly ‘minimalist’ approach which focuses on the cognitive aetiology of pragmatics. Pragmatics is seen as the consequence of interactions between a set of linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive subsystems which determine the crucial balance between how much information is encoded linguistically and how much is left unsaid on the grounds that it is recoverable from the linguistic and nonlinguistic context of utterance. Since it is often only when a complex system goes wrong that we become aware of the individual contributions of its component subsystems, my empirical evidence for the various cognitive entities discussed in this paper is the behaviour of people with communication disorders. Data from of a wide range of developmental and acquired language pathologies are presented in support of my argument. The approach adopted makes it possible to be more precise about the cognitive basis of pragmatics.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/S0378-2166(97)00055-6
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85646054</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0378216697000556</els_id><sourcerecordid>1518383518</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c461t-b31a7ee869692c43ba910b22e6e0bd331601be112b28c9cbd512821ccb3e10c43</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkEFLwzAYhoMoOKc_QSgIoodqvqRN09PQMXUw8KCeQ5N-04y2qUk78N_bbuLBi5fvuzzvC-9DyDnQG6Agbl8oz2TMQIirPLumlKZpLA7IBGSWx8Bldkgmv8gxOQlhM0CQcDoh98sQtb54r4vOmhBha9sPbFyNTVHNosXWltgYjNbe1ZFxdd031gyoa6LSBudL9OGUHK2LKuDZz5-St4fF6_wpXj0_Lud3q9gkArpYcygyRClykTOTcF3kQDVjKJDqknMQFDQCMM2kyY0uU2CSgTGaI9AhMCWX-97Wu88eQ6dqGwxWVdGg64OSqUgETUfw4g-4cb0fBgUFKUgu-XinJN1TxrsQPK5V621d-C8FVI1e1c6rGqWpPFM7r0oMudk-h8PWrUWvgrGjpNJ6NJ0qnf2n4RuPwH7a</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1518383518</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders</title><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Perkins, Michael R.</creator><creatorcontrib>Perkins, Michael R.</creatorcontrib><description>In recent years there has been a converging trend in the study of pragmatics, grammar, language acquisition and neurolinguistics which sees as secondary epiphenomena a wide range of phenomena which were once regarded as primary. Examples of this trend in pragmatics include relevance theory, which explains speech acts and implicatures in terms of a more fundamental principle of relevance, and conversation analysis, which sees notions such as ‘topic’ as by-products of conversational coherence. In this paper I adopt a similarly ‘minimalist’ approach which focuses on the cognitive aetiology of pragmatics. Pragmatics is seen as the consequence of interactions between a set of linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive subsystems which determine the crucial balance between how much information is encoded linguistically and how much is left unsaid on the grounds that it is recoverable from the linguistic and nonlinguistic context of utterance. Since it is often only when a complex system goes wrong that we become aware of the individual contributions of its component subsystems, my empirical evidence for the various cognitive entities discussed in this paper is the behaviour of people with communication disorders. Data from of a wide range of developmental and acquired language pathologies are presented in support of my argument. The approach adopted makes it possible to be more precise about the cognitive basis of pragmatics.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0378-2166</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1879-1387</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/S0378-2166(97)00055-6</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JPRADM</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V</publisher><ispartof>Journal of pragmatics, 1998, Vol.29 (3), p.291-311</ispartof><rights>1998</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c461t-b31a7ee869692c43ba910b22e6e0bd331601be112b28c9cbd512821ccb3e10c43</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c461t-b31a7ee869692c43ba910b22e6e0bd331601be112b28c9cbd512821ccb3e10c43</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(97)00055-6$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>315,781,785,3551,4025,27871,27925,27926,27927,45997</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Perkins, Michael R.</creatorcontrib><title>Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders</title><title>Journal of pragmatics</title><description>In recent years there has been a converging trend in the study of pragmatics, grammar, language acquisition and neurolinguistics which sees as secondary epiphenomena a wide range of phenomena which were once regarded as primary. Examples of this trend in pragmatics include relevance theory, which explains speech acts and implicatures in terms of a more fundamental principle of relevance, and conversation analysis, which sees notions such as ‘topic’ as by-products of conversational coherence. In this paper I adopt a similarly ‘minimalist’ approach which focuses on the cognitive aetiology of pragmatics. Pragmatics is seen as the consequence of interactions between a set of linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive subsystems which determine the crucial balance between how much information is encoded linguistically and how much is left unsaid on the grounds that it is recoverable from the linguistic and nonlinguistic context of utterance. Since it is often only when a complex system goes wrong that we become aware of the individual contributions of its component subsystems, my empirical evidence for the various cognitive entities discussed in this paper is the behaviour of people with communication disorders. Data from of a wide range of developmental and acquired language pathologies are presented in support of my argument. The approach adopted makes it possible to be more precise about the cognitive basis of pragmatics.</description><issn>0378-2166</issn><issn>1879-1387</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1998</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkEFLwzAYhoMoOKc_QSgIoodqvqRN09PQMXUw8KCeQ5N-04y2qUk78N_bbuLBi5fvuzzvC-9DyDnQG6Agbl8oz2TMQIirPLumlKZpLA7IBGSWx8Bldkgmv8gxOQlhM0CQcDoh98sQtb54r4vOmhBha9sPbFyNTVHNosXWltgYjNbe1ZFxdd031gyoa6LSBudL9OGUHK2LKuDZz5-St4fF6_wpXj0_Lud3q9gkArpYcygyRClykTOTcF3kQDVjKJDqknMQFDQCMM2kyY0uU2CSgTGaI9AhMCWX-97Wu88eQ6dqGwxWVdGg64OSqUgETUfw4g-4cb0fBgUFKUgu-XinJN1TxrsQPK5V621d-C8FVI1e1c6rGqWpPFM7r0oMudk-h8PWrUWvgrGjpNJ6NJ0qnf2n4RuPwH7a</recordid><startdate>1998</startdate><enddate>1998</enddate><creator>Perkins, Michael R.</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>North-Holland Pub. Co</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>GHEHK</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>1998</creationdate><title>Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders</title><author>Perkins, Michael R.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c461t-b31a7ee869692c43ba910b22e6e0bd331601be112b28c9cbd512821ccb3e10c43</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1998</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Perkins, Michael R.</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 08</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Journal of pragmatics</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Perkins, Michael R.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders</atitle><jtitle>Journal of pragmatics</jtitle><date>1998</date><risdate>1998</risdate><volume>29</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>291</spage><epage>311</epage><pages>291-311</pages><issn>0378-2166</issn><eissn>1879-1387</eissn><coden>JPRADM</coden><abstract>In recent years there has been a converging trend in the study of pragmatics, grammar, language acquisition and neurolinguistics which sees as secondary epiphenomena a wide range of phenomena which were once regarded as primary. Examples of this trend in pragmatics include relevance theory, which explains speech acts and implicatures in terms of a more fundamental principle of relevance, and conversation analysis, which sees notions such as ‘topic’ as by-products of conversational coherence. In this paper I adopt a similarly ‘minimalist’ approach which focuses on the cognitive aetiology of pragmatics. Pragmatics is seen as the consequence of interactions between a set of linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive subsystems which determine the crucial balance between how much information is encoded linguistically and how much is left unsaid on the grounds that it is recoverable from the linguistic and nonlinguistic context of utterance. Since it is often only when a complex system goes wrong that we become aware of the individual contributions of its component subsystems, my empirical evidence for the various cognitive entities discussed in this paper is the behaviour of people with communication disorders. Data from of a wide range of developmental and acquired language pathologies are presented in support of my argument. The approach adopted makes it possible to be more precise about the cognitive basis of pragmatics.</abstract><cop>Amsterdam</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><doi>10.1016/S0378-2166(97)00055-6</doi><tpages>21</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0378-2166
ispartof Journal of pragmatics, 1998, Vol.29 (3), p.291-311
issn 0378-2166
1879-1387
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85646054
source Periodicals Index Online; Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)
title Is pragmatics epiphenomenal? Evidence from communication disorders
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-18T13%3A00%3A58IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Is%20pragmatics%20epiphenomenal?%20Evidence%20from%20communication%20disorders&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20pragmatics&rft.au=Perkins,%20Michael%20R.&rft.date=1998&rft.volume=29&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=291&rft.epage=311&rft.pages=291-311&rft.issn=0378-2166&rft.eissn=1879-1387&rft.coden=JPRADM&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/S0378-2166(97)00055-6&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1518383518%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1518383518&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=S0378216697000556&rfr_iscdi=true