The functions of phonology in the acquisition of reading: lexical and sentence processing
It has been claimed (V. Coltheart, Laxon, Rickard, & Elton, 1988) that learners as well as skilled readers use phonology for multiple functions in reading-for-meaning tasks. This claim was examined using lexical decision and sentence evaluation tasks. It was found in the first experiment that th...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Memory & cognition 1995-11, Vol.23 (6), p.749-766 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 766 |
---|---|
container_issue | 6 |
container_start_page | 749 |
container_title | Memory & cognition |
container_volume | 23 |
creator | Johnston, R S Thompson, G B Fletcher-Flinn, C M Holligan, C |
description | It has been claimed (V. Coltheart, Laxon, Rickard, & Elton, 1988) that learners as well as skilled readers use phonology for multiple functions in reading-for-meaning tasks. This claim was examined using lexical decision and sentence evaluation tasks. It was found in the first experiment that the type of instruction learners had received determined whether there was prelexical use of phonology in responding to items out of sentence context. Type of instruction had no effect when the items were in context. In the second experiment, performances on a homophone sentence evaluation task and a homophone semantic decision task, which excluded sentence processing, were examined. The results suggest that phonology served the function of access to lexical meanings in addition to any function in postlexical sentence processing. The obtained relationships between relative frequencies of the presented and unpresented homophone mates and item accuracy on these tasks were inconsistent with exclusive use of "direct access" but consistent with access of lexical meaning via phonology and application of a "spelling-check" procedure when multiple homophonic meanings are activated. |
doi_str_mv | 10.3758/bf03200927 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85601332</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>8717927</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-a7801ba72253e8922e5f727961486f489393521b04e6974da07a252e57527e4c3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0U1LHEEQBuAmRMxqcsk90CTgQRhT_TXV7c0sfoHgxUByGnp7a3RktnudngH99_bimkNAPBVUPVRRvIx9FXCk0NifixaUBHASP7CZMEpWxun6I5uVHlQG5J9PbC_newAwxtW7bNcaZbXGGft7c0e8nWIYuxQzTy1f36WY-nT7xLvIxzL14WHqcrcBm_lAftnF22Pe02MXfM99XPJMcaQYiK-HFCjnAj6zndb3mb5s6z77fXZ6M7-orq7PL-cnV1UwIMbKowWx8CilUWSdlGRalOhqoW3dauuUU0aKBWiqHeqlB_TSFIVGIumg9tnBy95y-mGiPDarLgfqex8pTbmxpgahlHwXIqIBlPAuVNZqYYUu8Pt_8D5NQyzfNlKgNqhwg368hYR0StgSiSvq8EWFIeU8UNush27lh6dGQLMJufl19hpywd-2K6fFipb_6DZV9QzFvp4n</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1293185599</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The functions of phonology in the acquisition of reading: lexical and sentence processing</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>Johnston, R S ; Thompson, G B ; Fletcher-Flinn, C M ; Holligan, C</creator><creatorcontrib>Johnston, R S ; Thompson, G B ; Fletcher-Flinn, C M ; Holligan, C</creatorcontrib><description>It has been claimed (V. Coltheart, Laxon, Rickard, & Elton, 1988) that learners as well as skilled readers use phonology for multiple functions in reading-for-meaning tasks. This claim was examined using lexical decision and sentence evaluation tasks. It was found in the first experiment that the type of instruction learners had received determined whether there was prelexical use of phonology in responding to items out of sentence context. Type of instruction had no effect when the items were in context. In the second experiment, performances on a homophone sentence evaluation task and a homophone semantic decision task, which excluded sentence processing, were examined. The results suggest that phonology served the function of access to lexical meanings in addition to any function in postlexical sentence processing. The obtained relationships between relative frequencies of the presented and unpresented homophone mates and item accuracy on these tasks were inconsistent with exclusive use of "direct access" but consistent with access of lexical meaning via phonology and application of a "spelling-check" procedure when multiple homophonic meanings are activated.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0090-502X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1532-5946</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.3758/bf03200927</identifier><identifier>PMID: 8538447</identifier><identifier>CODEN: MYCGAO</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Psychonomic Society</publisher><subject>Attention ; Child ; Cognition & reasoning ; Communication disorders ; Female ; Humans ; Learning ; Male ; Mental Recall ; Phonetics ; Phonology ; Psychology ; Reading ; Reading habits ; Semantics ; Sentences ; Verbal Learning</subject><ispartof>Memory & cognition, 1995-11, Vol.23 (6), p.749-766</ispartof><rights>Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Nov 1995</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-a7801ba72253e8922e5f727961486f489393521b04e6974da07a252e57527e4c3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-a7801ba72253e8922e5f727961486f489393521b04e6974da07a252e57527e4c3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27868,27923,27924</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8538447$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Johnston, R S</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Thompson, G B</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fletcher-Flinn, C M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Holligan, C</creatorcontrib><title>The functions of phonology in the acquisition of reading: lexical and sentence processing</title><title>Memory & cognition</title><addtitle>Mem Cognit</addtitle><description>It has been claimed (V. Coltheart, Laxon, Rickard, & Elton, 1988) that learners as well as skilled readers use phonology for multiple functions in reading-for-meaning tasks. This claim was examined using lexical decision and sentence evaluation tasks. It was found in the first experiment that the type of instruction learners had received determined whether there was prelexical use of phonology in responding to items out of sentence context. Type of instruction had no effect when the items were in context. In the second experiment, performances on a homophone sentence evaluation task and a homophone semantic decision task, which excluded sentence processing, were examined. The results suggest that phonology served the function of access to lexical meanings in addition to any function in postlexical sentence processing. The obtained relationships between relative frequencies of the presented and unpresented homophone mates and item accuracy on these tasks were inconsistent with exclusive use of "direct access" but consistent with access of lexical meaning via phonology and application of a "spelling-check" procedure when multiple homophonic meanings are activated.</description><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Communication disorders</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mental Recall</subject><subject>Phonetics</subject><subject>Phonology</subject><subject>Psychology</subject><subject>Reading</subject><subject>Reading habits</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Sentences</subject><subject>Verbal Learning</subject><issn>0090-502X</issn><issn>1532-5946</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1995</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0U1LHEEQBuAmRMxqcsk90CTgQRhT_TXV7c0sfoHgxUByGnp7a3RktnudngH99_bimkNAPBVUPVRRvIx9FXCk0NifixaUBHASP7CZMEpWxun6I5uVHlQG5J9PbC_newAwxtW7bNcaZbXGGft7c0e8nWIYuxQzTy1f36WY-nT7xLvIxzL14WHqcrcBm_lAftnF22Pe02MXfM99XPJMcaQYiK-HFCjnAj6zndb3mb5s6z77fXZ6M7-orq7PL-cnV1UwIMbKowWx8CilUWSdlGRalOhqoW3dauuUU0aKBWiqHeqlB_TSFIVGIumg9tnBy95y-mGiPDarLgfqex8pTbmxpgahlHwXIqIBlPAuVNZqYYUu8Pt_8D5NQyzfNlKgNqhwg368hYR0StgSiSvq8EWFIeU8UNush27lh6dGQLMJufl19hpywd-2K6fFipb_6DZV9QzFvp4n</recordid><startdate>199511</startdate><enddate>199511</enddate><creator>Johnston, R S</creator><creator>Thompson, G B</creator><creator>Fletcher-Flinn, C M</creator><creator>Holligan, C</creator><general>Psychonomic Society</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>SDSKB</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>199511</creationdate><title>The functions of phonology in the acquisition of reading: lexical and sentence processing</title><author>Johnston, R S ; Thompson, G B ; Fletcher-Flinn, C M ; Holligan, C</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c501t-a7801ba72253e8922e5f727961486f489393521b04e6974da07a252e57527e4c3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1995</creationdate><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Communication disorders</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mental Recall</topic><topic>Phonetics</topic><topic>Phonology</topic><topic>Psychology</topic><topic>Reading</topic><topic>Reading habits</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Sentences</topic><topic>Verbal Learning</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Johnston, R S</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Thompson, G B</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fletcher-Flinn, C M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Holligan, C</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 43</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Memory & cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Johnston, R S</au><au>Thompson, G B</au><au>Fletcher-Flinn, C M</au><au>Holligan, C</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>The functions of phonology in the acquisition of reading: lexical and sentence processing</atitle><jtitle>Memory & cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Mem Cognit</addtitle><date>1995-11</date><risdate>1995</risdate><volume>23</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>749</spage><epage>766</epage><pages>749-766</pages><issn>0090-502X</issn><eissn>1532-5946</eissn><coden>MYCGAO</coden><abstract>It has been claimed (V. Coltheart, Laxon, Rickard, & Elton, 1988) that learners as well as skilled readers use phonology for multiple functions in reading-for-meaning tasks. This claim was examined using lexical decision and sentence evaluation tasks. It was found in the first experiment that the type of instruction learners had received determined whether there was prelexical use of phonology in responding to items out of sentence context. Type of instruction had no effect when the items were in context. In the second experiment, performances on a homophone sentence evaluation task and a homophone semantic decision task, which excluded sentence processing, were examined. The results suggest that phonology served the function of access to lexical meanings in addition to any function in postlexical sentence processing. The obtained relationships between relative frequencies of the presented and unpresented homophone mates and item accuracy on these tasks were inconsistent with exclusive use of "direct access" but consistent with access of lexical meaning via phonology and application of a "spelling-check" procedure when multiple homophonic meanings are activated.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Psychonomic Society</pub><pmid>8538447</pmid><doi>10.3758/bf03200927</doi><tpages>18</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0090-502X |
ispartof | Memory & cognition, 1995-11, Vol.23 (6), p.749-766 |
issn | 0090-502X 1532-5946 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85601332 |
source | MEDLINE; Periodicals Index Online; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings |
subjects | Attention Child Cognition & reasoning Communication disorders Female Humans Learning Male Mental Recall Phonetics Phonology Psychology Reading Reading habits Semantics Sentences Verbal Learning |
title | The functions of phonology in the acquisition of reading: lexical and sentence processing |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-09T10%3A00%3A20IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20functions%20of%20phonology%20in%20the%20acquisition%20of%20reading:%20lexical%20and%20sentence%20processing&rft.jtitle=Memory%20&%20cognition&rft.au=Johnston,%20R%20S&rft.date=1995-11&rft.volume=23&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=749&rft.epage=766&rft.pages=749-766&rft.issn=0090-502X&rft.eissn=1532-5946&rft.coden=MYCGAO&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758/bf03200927&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E8717927%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1293185599&rft_id=info:pmid/8538447&rfr_iscdi=true |