The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: an experimental study

Icelandic and Norwegian past tense morphology contain strong patterns of inflection and two weak patterns of inflection. We report the results of an elicitation task that tests Icelandic and Norwegian children's knowledge of the past tense forms of a representative sample of verbs. This cross-s...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Journal of child language 1999-10, Vol.26 (3), p.577-618
Hauptverfasser: RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR, SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM, PLUNKETT, KIM
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 618
container_issue 3
container_start_page 577
container_title Journal of child language
container_volume 26
creator RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR
SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM
PLUNKETT, KIM
description Icelandic and Norwegian past tense morphology contain strong patterns of inflection and two weak patterns of inflection. We report the results of an elicitation task that tests Icelandic and Norwegian children's knowledge of the past tense forms of a representative sample of verbs. This cross-sectional study of four-, six- and eight-year-old Icelandic (n=92) and Norwegian (n=96) children systematically manipulates verb characteristics such as type frequency, token frequency and phonological coherence – factors that are generally considered to have an important impact on the acquisition of inflectional morphology in other languages. Our findings confirm that these factors play an important role in the acquisition of Icelandic and Norwegian. In addition, the results indicate that the predominant source of errors in children shifts during the later stages of development from one weak verb class to the other. We conclude that these findings are consistent with the view that exemplar-based learning, whereby patterns of categorization and generalization are driven by similarity to known forms, appropriately characterizes the acquisition of inflectional systems by Icelandic and Norwegian children.
doi_str_mv 10.1017/S0305000999003918
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85517570</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S0305000999003918</cupid><ericid>EJ597362</ericid><sourcerecordid>69366584</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-18f4210d49b08a73042e0becc8217069b9768448e257fae9490c44ac2808f973</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqNkU9v1DAQxSMEotvCB0BCyBIVt8A4_t8bWsq2qAIh9sDNchxn1yWbpHYiut8eR1mVCoTKxbb8fvM0My_LXmB4iwGLd9-AAAMApRQAUVg-yhaYcpULDsXjbDHJ-aQfZccxXk8vUPJpdoSBA-FKLDK73jpk7M3oox9816KuRr2JAxpcGx3adaHfdk232SPfokvrGtNW3qJ0os9d-Ok23rTIbn1TBdeepX_kbnsX_M61g2lQHMZq_yx7UpsmuueH-yRbfzxfLy_yqy-ry-X7q9wyRYYcy5oWGCqqSpBGEKCFg9JZKwssgKtSCS4pla5gojZOUQWWUmMLCbJWgpxkb2bbPnQ3o4uD3vmYOk4tu26MWjKGBRPwH2ChKFb8QZArwjmT9EGQCUG5BJzA13-A190Y2rQVjUUKkOEUUKLwTNnQxRhcrfu0URP2GoOektd_JZ9qXh2cx3LnqnsVc9QJOD0AJlrT1MG01sffnFKckQl7OWMpRHunnn9iyYIXSc5n2cfB3d7pJvzQXBDBNF991RcflivyfSW0Sjw5jGJ2ZfDVxt2b-J_D_AIpH9q_</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1791851098</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: an experimental study</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Cambridge University Press Journals Complete</source><creator>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR ; SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM ; PLUNKETT, KIM</creator><creatorcontrib>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR ; SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM ; PLUNKETT, KIM</creatorcontrib><description>Icelandic and Norwegian past tense morphology contain strong patterns of inflection and two weak patterns of inflection. We report the results of an elicitation task that tests Icelandic and Norwegian children's knowledge of the past tense forms of a representative sample of verbs. This cross-sectional study of four-, six- and eight-year-old Icelandic (n=92) and Norwegian (n=96) children systematically manipulates verb characteristics such as type frequency, token frequency and phonological coherence – factors that are generally considered to have an important impact on the acquisition of inflectional morphology in other languages. Our findings confirm that these factors play an important role in the acquisition of Icelandic and Norwegian. In addition, the results indicate that the predominant source of errors in children shifts during the later stages of development from one weak verb class to the other. We conclude that these findings are consistent with the view that exemplar-based learning, whereby patterns of categorization and generalization are driven by similarity to known forms, appropriately characterizes the acquisition of inflectional systems by Icelandic and Norwegian children.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0305-0009</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-7602</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0305000999003918</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10603697</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JCLGBJ</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>NewYork, NY: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>Acquisition ; Biological and medical sciences ; Child ; Child development ; Child Language ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Developmental psychology ; Female ; Foreign Countries ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Grammar ; Humans ; Iceland ; Icelandic ; Icelandic language ; Language ; Language Acquisition ; Male ; Morphology ; Morphology (Languages) ; Norway ; Norwegian ; Norwegian language ; Past Tense ; Production and perception of spoken language ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Verbal Learning ; Verbs ; Young children</subject><ispartof>Journal of child language, 1999-10, Vol.26 (3), p.577-618</ispartof><rights>1999 Cambridge University Press</rights><rights>1999 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-18f4210d49b08a73042e0becc8217069b9768448e257fae9490c44ac2808f973</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0305000999003918/type/journal_article$$EHTML$$P50$$Gcambridge$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>164,314,780,784,27869,27924,27925,31000,55628</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ597362$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=1996537$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10603697$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>PLUNKETT, KIM</creatorcontrib><title>The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: an experimental study</title><title>Journal of child language</title><addtitle>J. Child Lang</addtitle><description>Icelandic and Norwegian past tense morphology contain strong patterns of inflection and two weak patterns of inflection. We report the results of an elicitation task that tests Icelandic and Norwegian children's knowledge of the past tense forms of a representative sample of verbs. This cross-sectional study of four-, six- and eight-year-old Icelandic (n=92) and Norwegian (n=96) children systematically manipulates verb characteristics such as type frequency, token frequency and phonological coherence – factors that are generally considered to have an important impact on the acquisition of inflectional morphology in other languages. Our findings confirm that these factors play an important role in the acquisition of Icelandic and Norwegian. In addition, the results indicate that the predominant source of errors in children shifts during the later stages of development from one weak verb class to the other. We conclude that these findings are consistent with the view that exemplar-based learning, whereby patterns of categorization and generalization are driven by similarity to known forms, appropriately characterizes the acquisition of inflectional systems by Icelandic and Norwegian children.</description><subject>Acquisition</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Child Language</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Foreign Countries</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Grammar</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Iceland</subject><subject>Icelandic</subject><subject>Icelandic language</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language Acquisition</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Morphology</subject><subject>Morphology (Languages)</subject><subject>Norway</subject><subject>Norwegian</subject><subject>Norwegian language</subject><subject>Past Tense</subject><subject>Production and perception of spoken language</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Verbal Learning</subject><subject>Verbs</subject><subject>Young children</subject><issn>0305-0009</issn><issn>1469-7602</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1999</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqNkU9v1DAQxSMEotvCB0BCyBIVt8A4_t8bWsq2qAIh9sDNchxn1yWbpHYiut8eR1mVCoTKxbb8fvM0My_LXmB4iwGLd9-AAAMApRQAUVg-yhaYcpULDsXjbDHJ-aQfZccxXk8vUPJpdoSBA-FKLDK73jpk7M3oox9816KuRr2JAxpcGx3adaHfdk232SPfokvrGtNW3qJ0os9d-Ok23rTIbn1TBdeepX_kbnsX_M61g2lQHMZq_yx7UpsmuueH-yRbfzxfLy_yqy-ry-X7q9wyRYYcy5oWGCqqSpBGEKCFg9JZKwssgKtSCS4pla5gojZOUQWWUmMLCbJWgpxkb2bbPnQ3o4uD3vmYOk4tu26MWjKGBRPwH2ChKFb8QZArwjmT9EGQCUG5BJzA13-A190Y2rQVjUUKkOEUUKLwTNnQxRhcrfu0URP2GoOektd_JZ9qXh2cx3LnqnsVc9QJOD0AJlrT1MG01sffnFKckQl7OWMpRHunnn9iyYIXSc5n2cfB3d7pJvzQXBDBNF991RcflivyfSW0Sjw5jGJ2ZfDVxt2b-J_D_AIpH9q_</recordid><startdate>19991001</startdate><enddate>19991001</enddate><creator>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR</creator><creator>SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM</creator><creator>PLUNKETT, KIM</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>HJHVS</scope><scope>HQAFP</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>8BM</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19991001</creationdate><title>The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: an experimental study</title><author>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR ; SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM ; PLUNKETT, KIM</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c593t-18f4210d49b08a73042e0becc8217069b9768448e257fae9490c44ac2808f973</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1999</creationdate><topic>Acquisition</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Child Language</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Foreign Countries</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Grammar</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Iceland</topic><topic>Icelandic</topic><topic>Icelandic language</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language Acquisition</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Morphology</topic><topic>Morphology (Languages)</topic><topic>Norway</topic><topic>Norwegian</topic><topic>Norwegian language</topic><topic>Past Tense</topic><topic>Production and perception of spoken language</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Verbal Learning</topic><topic>Verbs</topic><topic>Young children</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>PLUNKETT, KIM</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 19</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 23</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Journal of child language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>RAGNARSDÓTTIR, HRAFNHILDUR</au><au>SIMONSEN, HANNE GRAM</au><au>PLUNKETT, KIM</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ597362</ericid><atitle>The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: an experimental study</atitle><jtitle>Journal of child language</jtitle><addtitle>J. Child Lang</addtitle><date>1999-10-01</date><risdate>1999</risdate><volume>26</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>577</spage><epage>618</epage><pages>577-618</pages><issn>0305-0009</issn><eissn>1469-7602</eissn><coden>JCLGBJ</coden><abstract>Icelandic and Norwegian past tense morphology contain strong patterns of inflection and two weak patterns of inflection. We report the results of an elicitation task that tests Icelandic and Norwegian children's knowledge of the past tense forms of a representative sample of verbs. This cross-sectional study of four-, six- and eight-year-old Icelandic (n=92) and Norwegian (n=96) children systematically manipulates verb characteristics such as type frequency, token frequency and phonological coherence – factors that are generally considered to have an important impact on the acquisition of inflectional morphology in other languages. Our findings confirm that these factors play an important role in the acquisition of Icelandic and Norwegian. In addition, the results indicate that the predominant source of errors in children shifts during the later stages of development from one weak verb class to the other. We conclude that these findings are consistent with the view that exemplar-based learning, whereby patterns of categorization and generalization are driven by similarity to known forms, appropriately characterizes the acquisition of inflectional systems by Icelandic and Norwegian children.</abstract><cop>NewYork, NY</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><pmid>10603697</pmid><doi>10.1017/S0305000999003918</doi><tpages>42</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0305-0009
ispartof Journal of child language, 1999-10, Vol.26 (3), p.577-618
issn 0305-0009
1469-7602
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85517570
source MEDLINE; Periodicals Index Online; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Cambridge University Press Journals Complete
subjects Acquisition
Biological and medical sciences
Child
Child development
Child Language
Child, Preschool
Children
Developmental psychology
Female
Foreign Countries
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Grammar
Humans
Iceland
Icelandic
Icelandic language
Language
Language Acquisition
Male
Morphology
Morphology (Languages)
Norway
Norwegian
Norwegian language
Past Tense
Production and perception of spoken language
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Verbal Learning
Verbs
Young children
title The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: an experimental study
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-06T23%3A19%3A49IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The%20acquisition%20of%20past%20tense%20morphology%20in%20Icelandic%20and%20Norwegian%20children:%20an%20experimental%20study&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20child%20language&rft.au=RAGNARSD%C3%93TTIR,%20HRAFNHILDUR&rft.date=1999-10-01&rft.volume=26&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=577&rft.epage=618&rft.pages=577-618&rft.issn=0305-0009&rft.eissn=1469-7602&rft.coden=JCLGBJ&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0305000999003918&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E69366584%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1791851098&rft_id=info:pmid/10603697&rft_cupid=10_1017_S0305000999003918&rft_ericid=EJ597362&rfr_iscdi=true