Was the item recalled and if so by whom?
In a single-trial free recall paradigm using 10 categorized or 10 random word lists, two individuals (Self and Other) alternatively recalled the items of each list. Subsequently they judged (a) which items were recalled and (b) which person (Self or Other) recalled the items judged as having been re...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of memory and language 1987-08, Vol.26 (4), p.466-479 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 479 |
---|---|
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | 466 |
container_title | Journal of memory and language |
container_volume | 26 |
creator | Voss, James F Vesonder, Gregg T Post, Timothy A Ney, Laurie G |
description | In a single-trial free recall paradigm using 10 categorized or 10 random word lists, two individuals (Self and Other) alternatively recalled the items of each list. Subsequently they judged (a) which items were recalled and (b) which person (Self or Other) recalled the items judged as having been recalled. A standard, single-subject free recall paradigm was also employed. The results indicated that Self correctly judged an item as having beer recalled with a higher probability than judging items that Other recalled, supporting the generation effect (
Slamecka & Graf, 1978). Judgment of item recall was thus presumed due to response-produced cues related to item recall by Other and by Self as well as to cues generated by Self in the process of recalling the item. In addition, source identification (Self or Other) for items judged as recalled yielded 75–80% accuracy, and although identification of which person recalled the item was not significantly different for Self-recalled and Other-recalled items, additional results supported Self's use of a strategy in which Self first sought cues related to his/her own recall of an item and responded “self” if the cues were found and “other” if they were not found. The results are discussed in relation to the generation effect and to the concept of reality monitoring. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90102-1 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85488131</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>0749596X87901021</els_id><sourcerecordid>1297346830</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a385t-e42d003b50185148621e6fa87484732f1ad8db0b12a189f4f680f3df7a49b1d43</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kE1LxDAQQIMouK7-Aw8BQfRQnUnSNr0osvgFC14UvYW0Sdgs_ViTrrL_3tYVDx48zeXNY-YRcoxwgYDZJeSiSNIiezuT-XkBCCzBHTJBKLIEJMNdMvlF9slBjEsAxDRnE3L2qiPtF5b63jY02ErXtTVUt4Z6R2NHyw39XHTN9SHZc7qO9uhnTsnL3e3z7CGZP90_zm7mieYy7RMrmAHgZQooUxQyY2gzp2UupMg5c6iNNCWUyDTKwgmXSXDcuFyLokQj-JScbr2r0L2vbexV42Nl61q3tltHJVMhJXIcwJM_4LJbh3a4TSErci4yyWGgxJaqQhdjsE6tgm902CgENcZTYxk1llEyV9_x1Ci_2q7Z4dUPb4OKlbdtZY0fGvXKdP5_wRcgSnKs</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1297346830</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Was the item recalled and if so by whom?</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Voss, James F ; Vesonder, Gregg T ; Post, Timothy A ; Ney, Laurie G</creator><creatorcontrib>Voss, James F ; Vesonder, Gregg T ; Post, Timothy A ; Ney, Laurie G</creatorcontrib><description>In a single-trial free recall paradigm using 10 categorized or 10 random word lists, two individuals (Self and Other) alternatively recalled the items of each list. Subsequently they judged (a) which items were recalled and (b) which person (Self or Other) recalled the items judged as having been recalled. A standard, single-subject free recall paradigm was also employed. The results indicated that Self correctly judged an item as having beer recalled with a higher probability than judging items that Other recalled, supporting the generation effect (
Slamecka & Graf, 1978). Judgment of item recall was thus presumed due to response-produced cues related to item recall by Other and by Self as well as to cues generated by Self in the process of recalling the item. In addition, source identification (Self or Other) for items judged as recalled yielded 75–80% accuracy, and although identification of which person recalled the item was not significantly different for Self-recalled and Other-recalled items, additional results supported Self's use of a strategy in which Self first sought cues related to his/her own recall of an item and responded “self” if the cues were found and “other” if they were not found. The results are discussed in relation to the generation effect and to the concept of reality monitoring.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0749-596X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1096-0821</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90102-1</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JMLAE6</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York: Elsevier Inc</publisher><ispartof>Journal of memory and language, 1987-08, Vol.26 (4), p.466-479</ispartof><rights>1987</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a385t-e42d003b50185148621e6fa87484732f1ad8db0b12a189f4f680f3df7a49b1d43</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-a385t-e42d003b50185148621e6fa87484732f1ad8db0b12a189f4f680f3df7a49b1d43</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0749596X87901021$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,27846,27901,27902,65306</link.rule.ids></links><search><creatorcontrib>Voss, James F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vesonder, Gregg T</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Post, Timothy A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ney, Laurie G</creatorcontrib><title>Was the item recalled and if so by whom?</title><title>Journal of memory and language</title><description>In a single-trial free recall paradigm using 10 categorized or 10 random word lists, two individuals (Self and Other) alternatively recalled the items of each list. Subsequently they judged (a) which items were recalled and (b) which person (Self or Other) recalled the items judged as having been recalled. A standard, single-subject free recall paradigm was also employed. The results indicated that Self correctly judged an item as having beer recalled with a higher probability than judging items that Other recalled, supporting the generation effect (
Slamecka & Graf, 1978). Judgment of item recall was thus presumed due to response-produced cues related to item recall by Other and by Self as well as to cues generated by Self in the process of recalling the item. In addition, source identification (Self or Other) for items judged as recalled yielded 75–80% accuracy, and although identification of which person recalled the item was not significantly different for Self-recalled and Other-recalled items, additional results supported Self's use of a strategy in which Self first sought cues related to his/her own recall of an item and responded “self” if the cues were found and “other” if they were not found. The results are discussed in relation to the generation effect and to the concept of reality monitoring.</description><issn>0749-596X</issn><issn>1096-0821</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1987</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>0R3</sourceid><sourceid>ACFII</sourceid><sourceid>HYQOX</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><sourceid>~OC</sourceid><sourceid>~PJ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kE1LxDAQQIMouK7-Aw8BQfRQnUnSNr0osvgFC14UvYW0Sdgs_ViTrrL_3tYVDx48zeXNY-YRcoxwgYDZJeSiSNIiezuT-XkBCCzBHTJBKLIEJMNdMvlF9slBjEsAxDRnE3L2qiPtF5b63jY02ErXtTVUt4Z6R2NHyw39XHTN9SHZc7qO9uhnTsnL3e3z7CGZP90_zm7mieYy7RMrmAHgZQooUxQyY2gzp2UupMg5c6iNNCWUyDTKwgmXSXDcuFyLokQj-JScbr2r0L2vbexV42Nl61q3tltHJVMhJXIcwJM_4LJbh3a4TSErci4yyWGgxJaqQhdjsE6tgm902CgENcZTYxk1llEyV9_x1Ci_2q7Z4dUPb4OKlbdtZY0fGvXKdP5_wRcgSnKs</recordid><startdate>19870801</startdate><enddate>19870801</enddate><creator>Voss, James F</creator><creator>Vesonder, Gregg T</creator><creator>Post, Timothy A</creator><creator>Ney, Laurie G</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Academic Press</general><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0R3</scope><scope>ACFII</scope><scope>ANHVI</scope><scope>FUVTR</scope><scope>HYQOX</scope><scope>IBDFT</scope><scope>JSICY</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>SFNNT</scope><scope>~OB</scope><scope>~OC</scope><scope>~OG</scope><scope>~PJ</scope><scope>~PM</scope><scope>~PN</scope><scope>7T9</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19870801</creationdate><title>Was the item recalled and if so by whom?</title><author>Voss, James F ; Vesonder, Gregg T ; Post, Timothy A ; Ney, Laurie G</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a385t-e42d003b50185148621e6fa87484732f1ad8db0b12a189f4f680f3df7a49b1d43</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1987</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Voss, James F</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Vesonder, Gregg T</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Post, Timothy A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Ney, Laurie G</creatorcontrib><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Collection 1.2</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Foundation Collection 1 (2022)</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Liberal Arts Collection 1 (2022)</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 06</collection><collection>ProQuest Historical Periodicals</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 27</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 36</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 44</collection><collection>PAO Collection 1</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Collection 1</collection><collection>PAO Collection 1 (purchase pre Feb/2008)</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Foundation Collection</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online JISC Collection</collection><collection>Periodicals Archive Online Liberal Arts Collection 1</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><jtitle>Journal of memory and language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Voss, James F</au><au>Vesonder, Gregg T</au><au>Post, Timothy A</au><au>Ney, Laurie G</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Was the item recalled and if so by whom?</atitle><jtitle>Journal of memory and language</jtitle><date>1987-08-01</date><risdate>1987</risdate><volume>26</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>466</spage><epage>479</epage><pages>466-479</pages><issn>0749-596X</issn><eissn>1096-0821</eissn><coden>JMLAE6</coden><abstract>In a single-trial free recall paradigm using 10 categorized or 10 random word lists, two individuals (Self and Other) alternatively recalled the items of each list. Subsequently they judged (a) which items were recalled and (b) which person (Self or Other) recalled the items judged as having been recalled. A standard, single-subject free recall paradigm was also employed. The results indicated that Self correctly judged an item as having beer recalled with a higher probability than judging items that Other recalled, supporting the generation effect (
Slamecka & Graf, 1978). Judgment of item recall was thus presumed due to response-produced cues related to item recall by Other and by Self as well as to cues generated by Self in the process of recalling the item. In addition, source identification (Self or Other) for items judged as recalled yielded 75–80% accuracy, and although identification of which person recalled the item was not significantly different for Self-recalled and Other-recalled items, additional results supported Self's use of a strategy in which Self first sought cues related to his/her own recall of an item and responded “self” if the cues were found and “other” if they were not found. The results are discussed in relation to the generation effect and to the concept of reality monitoring.</abstract><cop>New York</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><doi>10.1016/0749-596X(87)90102-1</doi><tpages>14</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0749-596X |
ispartof | Journal of memory and language, 1987-08, Vol.26 (4), p.466-479 |
issn | 0749-596X 1096-0821 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85488131 |
source | Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals; Alma/SFX Local Collection; Periodicals Index Online |
title | Was the item recalled and if so by whom? |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-03T12%3A00%3A11IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Was%20the%20item%20recalled%20and%20if%20so%20by%20whom?&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20memory%20and%20language&rft.au=Voss,%20James%20F&rft.date=1987-08-01&rft.volume=26&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=466&rft.epage=479&rft.pages=466-479&rft.issn=0749-596X&rft.eissn=1096-0821&rft.coden=JMLAE6&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/0749-596X(87)90102-1&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1297346830%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1297346830&rft_id=info:pmid/&rft_els_id=0749596X87901021&rfr_iscdi=true |