Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[]
What is the input to the mental System that computes inflected forms like walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a word's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to generalize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stopped, step-stepp...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of child language 1994-02, Vol.21 (1), p.173-209 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 209 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 173 |
container_title | Journal of child language |
container_volume | 21 |
creator | Kim, John J. Marcus, Gary F. Pinker, Steven Hollander, Michelle Coppola, Marie |
description | What is the input to the mental System that computes inflected forms like walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a word's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to generalize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stopped, step-stepped and fling-flung, cling-clung. But for adults, phonological input is insufficient: verbs derived from nouns like ring the city always have regular past tense forms (ringed), even if they are phonologically identical to irregular verbs (ring the bell). Similarly, nouns based on names, like two Mickey Mouses, and compounds based on possessing rather than being their root morpheme, such as two sabertooths, take regular plurals, even when they are homophonous with irregular nouns like mice and teeth. In four experiments, testing 70 three- to ten-year-old children, we found that children are sensitive to such nonphonological information: they were more likely to produce regular inflected forms for forms like to ring (‘to put a ring on’) and snaggletooth (a kind of animal doll with big teeth) than for their homophonous irregular counterparts, even when these counterparts were also extended in meaning. Children's inflectional Systems thus seem to be like adults': irregular forms are tied to the lexicon but regular forms are computed by a default rule, and words are represented as morphological tree structures reflecting their derivation from basic word roots. Such structures, which determine how novel complex words are derived and interpreted, also govern whether words with irregular sound patterns will be regularized: a word can be irregular only if its structure contains an irregular root in ‘head’ position, allowing the lexically stored irregular information to percolate up to apply to the word as a whole. In all other cases, the inflected form is computed by a default regular rule. This proposal fits the facts better than alternatives appealing to ambiguity reduction or semantic similarity to a word's central sense. The results, together with an analysis of adult speech to children, suggest that morphological structure and a distinction between mechanisms for regular and irregular inflection may be inherent to the design of children's language Systems. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1017/S0305000900008710 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85314147</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S0305000900008710</cupid><ericid>EJ491174</ericid><sourcerecordid>76564413</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c615t-2c7671ea1dc69c18b4d747f18c3b3e499b16df138ef71646ea5e6ff49fb625823</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkV1rFTEQhoMo9bT6AwSFBYterWY2X5srkVqrh4LUqjciIZtNatr9qElW7L8327McRJFzEXLxPDOTyYvQI8AvAIN4eY4JZhhjmQ-uBeA7aAWUy1JwXN1FqxmXM7-P9mO8vDVlvYf2aow5lrBCr87tEH3yP326KUZXmO--a4MdnsfCD66zJvlxKNJYXATd9zp5o7sipjCZNAX79dsDdM_pLtqHy32APr89_nT0rjz9cPL-6PVpaTiwVFZGcAFWQ2u4NFA3tBVUOKgNaYilUjbAWwektk4Ap9xqZrlzVLqGV6yuyAF6tul7HcYfk41J9T4a23V6sOMUVc0IUKBipyg445QC2SnmqVhW2dwpClZjQubRT_8SL8cpDPlbFJAchSRU0GzBxjJhjDFYp66D73W4UYDVnKr6J9Vc82TpPDW9bbcVS4yZHy5cxxyQC3owPm41IrPF5jaPN5oN3mzp8ZpKgNuXlRvsY7K_tlyHK8UFEUzxkzMlz958XEu2Vl-yT5ZNdN8E317YPxb-7y6_AYV7zlY</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1303093474</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[]</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Cambridge Journals</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Kim, John J. ; Marcus, Gary F. ; Pinker, Steven ; Hollander, Michelle ; Coppola, Marie</creator><creatorcontrib>Kim, John J. ; Marcus, Gary F. ; Pinker, Steven ; Hollander, Michelle ; Coppola, Marie</creatorcontrib><description>What is the input to the mental System that computes inflected forms like walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a word's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to generalize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stopped, step-stepped and fling-flung, cling-clung. But for adults, phonological input is insufficient: verbs derived from nouns like ring the city always have regular past tense forms (ringed), even if they are phonologically identical to irregular verbs (ring the bell). Similarly, nouns based on names, like two Mickey Mouses, and compounds based on possessing rather than being their root morpheme, such as two sabertooths, take regular plurals, even when they are homophonous with irregular nouns like mice and teeth. In four experiments, testing 70 three- to ten-year-old children, we found that children are sensitive to such nonphonological information: they were more likely to produce regular inflected forms for forms like to ring (‘to put a ring on’) and snaggletooth (a kind of animal doll with big teeth) than for their homophonous irregular counterparts, even when these counterparts were also extended in meaning. Children's inflectional Systems thus seem to be like adults': irregular forms are tied to the lexicon but regular forms are computed by a default rule, and words are represented as morphological tree structures reflecting their derivation from basic word roots. Such structures, which determine how novel complex words are derived and interpreted, also govern whether words with irregular sound patterns will be regularized: a word can be irregular only if its structure contains an irregular root in ‘head’ position, allowing the lexically stored irregular information to percolate up to apply to the word as a whole. In all other cases, the inflected form is computed by a default regular rule. This proposal fits the facts better than alternatives appealing to ambiguity reduction or semantic similarity to a word's central sense. The results, together with an analysis of adult speech to children, suggest that morphological structure and a distinction between mechanisms for regular and irregular inflection may be inherent to the design of children's language Systems.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0305-0009</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-7602</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0305000900008710</identifier><identifier>PMID: 8006091</identifier><identifier>CODEN: JCLGBJ</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>Biological and medical sciences ; Child ; Child development ; Child Language ; Child language. Acquisition and development ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Communication disorders ; Developmental psychology ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Grammar ; Homophones ; Humans ; Inflection (Grammar) ; Language Acquisition ; Language Development ; Linguistics ; Male ; Morphology (Languages) ; Nouns ; Phonetics ; Phonology ; Production and comprehension processes ; Psycholinguistics ; Psychology of language ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Reference Values ; Semantics ; Sensitivity ; Speech Acoustics ; Structural Analysis (Linguistics) ; Structure ; Verbs ; Young Children</subject><ispartof>Journal of child language, 1994-02, Vol.21 (1), p.173-209</ispartof><rights>Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994</rights><rights>1994 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c615t-2c7671ea1dc69c18b4d747f18c3b3e499b16df138ef71646ea5e6ff49fb625823</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c615t-2c7671ea1dc69c18b4d747f18c3b3e499b16df138ef71646ea5e6ff49fb625823</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0305000900008710/type/journal_article$$EHTML$$P50$$Gcambridge$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>164,314,776,780,27846,27901,27902,30977,55603</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ491174$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=3960950$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8006091$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Kim, John J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marcus, Gary F.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pinker, Steven</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hollander, Michelle</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Coppola, Marie</creatorcontrib><title>Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[]</title><title>Journal of child language</title><addtitle>J. Child Lang</addtitle><description>What is the input to the mental System that computes inflected forms like walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a word's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to generalize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stopped, step-stepped and fling-flung, cling-clung. But for adults, phonological input is insufficient: verbs derived from nouns like ring the city always have regular past tense forms (ringed), even if they are phonologically identical to irregular verbs (ring the bell). Similarly, nouns based on names, like two Mickey Mouses, and compounds based on possessing rather than being their root morpheme, such as two sabertooths, take regular plurals, even when they are homophonous with irregular nouns like mice and teeth. In four experiments, testing 70 three- to ten-year-old children, we found that children are sensitive to such nonphonological information: they were more likely to produce regular inflected forms for forms like to ring (‘to put a ring on’) and snaggletooth (a kind of animal doll with big teeth) than for their homophonous irregular counterparts, even when these counterparts were also extended in meaning. Children's inflectional Systems thus seem to be like adults': irregular forms are tied to the lexicon but regular forms are computed by a default rule, and words are represented as morphological tree structures reflecting their derivation from basic word roots. Such structures, which determine how novel complex words are derived and interpreted, also govern whether words with irregular sound patterns will be regularized: a word can be irregular only if its structure contains an irregular root in ‘head’ position, allowing the lexically stored irregular information to percolate up to apply to the word as a whole. In all other cases, the inflected form is computed by a default regular rule. This proposal fits the facts better than alternatives appealing to ambiguity reduction or semantic similarity to a word's central sense. The results, together with an analysis of adult speech to children, suggest that morphological structure and a distinction between mechanisms for regular and irregular inflection may be inherent to the design of children's language Systems.</description><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Child Language</subject><subject>Child language. Acquisition and development</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Communication disorders</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Grammar</subject><subject>Homophones</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Inflection (Grammar)</subject><subject>Language Acquisition</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>Linguistics</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Morphology (Languages)</subject><subject>Nouns</subject><subject>Phonetics</subject><subject>Phonology</subject><subject>Production and comprehension processes</subject><subject>Psycholinguistics</subject><subject>Psychology of language</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Reference Values</subject><subject>Semantics</subject><subject>Sensitivity</subject><subject>Speech Acoustics</subject><subject>Structural Analysis (Linguistics)</subject><subject>Structure</subject><subject>Verbs</subject><subject>Young Children</subject><issn>0305-0009</issn><issn>1469-7602</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1994</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkV1rFTEQhoMo9bT6AwSFBYterWY2X5srkVqrh4LUqjciIZtNatr9qElW7L8327McRJFzEXLxPDOTyYvQI8AvAIN4eY4JZhhjmQ-uBeA7aAWUy1JwXN1FqxmXM7-P9mO8vDVlvYf2aow5lrBCr87tEH3yP326KUZXmO--a4MdnsfCD66zJvlxKNJYXATd9zp5o7sipjCZNAX79dsDdM_pLtqHy32APr89_nT0rjz9cPL-6PVpaTiwVFZGcAFWQ2u4NFA3tBVUOKgNaYilUjbAWwektk4Ap9xqZrlzVLqGV6yuyAF6tul7HcYfk41J9T4a23V6sOMUVc0IUKBipyg445QC2SnmqVhW2dwpClZjQubRT_8SL8cpDPlbFJAchSRU0GzBxjJhjDFYp66D73W4UYDVnKr6J9Vc82TpPDW9bbcVS4yZHy5cxxyQC3owPm41IrPF5jaPN5oN3mzp8ZpKgNuXlRvsY7K_tlyHK8UFEUzxkzMlz958XEu2Vl-yT5ZNdN8E317YPxb-7y6_AYV7zlY</recordid><startdate>19940201</startdate><enddate>19940201</enddate><creator>Kim, John J.</creator><creator>Marcus, Gary F.</creator><creator>Pinker, Steven</creator><creator>Hollander, Michelle</creator><creator>Coppola, Marie</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>HJHVS</scope><scope>HQAFP</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7T9</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>8BM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19940201</creationdate><title>Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[]</title><author>Kim, John J. ; Marcus, Gary F. ; Pinker, Steven ; Hollander, Michelle ; Coppola, Marie</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c615t-2c7671ea1dc69c18b4d747f18c3b3e499b16df138ef71646ea5e6ff49fb625823</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1994</creationdate><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Child Language</topic><topic>Child language. Acquisition and development</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Communication disorders</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Grammar</topic><topic>Homophones</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Inflection (Grammar)</topic><topic>Language Acquisition</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>Linguistics</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Morphology (Languages)</topic><topic>Nouns</topic><topic>Phonetics</topic><topic>Phonology</topic><topic>Production and comprehension processes</topic><topic>Psycholinguistics</topic><topic>Psychology of language</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Reference Values</topic><topic>Semantics</topic><topic>Sensitivity</topic><topic>Speech Acoustics</topic><topic>Structural Analysis (Linguistics)</topic><topic>Structure</topic><topic>Verbs</topic><topic>Young Children</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Kim, John J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Marcus, Gary F.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pinker, Steven</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hollander, Michelle</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Coppola, Marie</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 19</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 23</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><jtitle>Journal of child language</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Kim, John J.</au><au>Marcus, Gary F.</au><au>Pinker, Steven</au><au>Hollander, Michelle</au><au>Coppola, Marie</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ491174</ericid><atitle>Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[]</atitle><jtitle>Journal of child language</jtitle><addtitle>J. Child Lang</addtitle><date>1994-02-01</date><risdate>1994</risdate><volume>21</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>173</spage><epage>209</epage><pages>173-209</pages><issn>0305-0009</issn><eissn>1469-7602</eissn><coden>JCLGBJ</coden><abstract>What is the input to the mental System that computes inflected forms like walked, came, dogs, and men? Recent connectionist models feed a word's phonological features into a single network, allowing it to generalize both regular and irregular phonological patterns, like stop-stopped, step-stepped and fling-flung, cling-clung. But for adults, phonological input is insufficient: verbs derived from nouns like ring the city always have regular past tense forms (ringed), even if they are phonologically identical to irregular verbs (ring the bell). Similarly, nouns based on names, like two Mickey Mouses, and compounds based on possessing rather than being their root morpheme, such as two sabertooths, take regular plurals, even when they are homophonous with irregular nouns like mice and teeth. In four experiments, testing 70 three- to ten-year-old children, we found that children are sensitive to such nonphonological information: they were more likely to produce regular inflected forms for forms like to ring (‘to put a ring on’) and snaggletooth (a kind of animal doll with big teeth) than for their homophonous irregular counterparts, even when these counterparts were also extended in meaning. Children's inflectional Systems thus seem to be like adults': irregular forms are tied to the lexicon but regular forms are computed by a default rule, and words are represented as morphological tree structures reflecting their derivation from basic word roots. Such structures, which determine how novel complex words are derived and interpreted, also govern whether words with irregular sound patterns will be regularized: a word can be irregular only if its structure contains an irregular root in ‘head’ position, allowing the lexically stored irregular information to percolate up to apply to the word as a whole. In all other cases, the inflected form is computed by a default regular rule. This proposal fits the facts better than alternatives appealing to ambiguity reduction or semantic similarity to a word's central sense. The results, together with an analysis of adult speech to children, suggest that morphological structure and a distinction between mechanisms for regular and irregular inflection may be inherent to the design of children's language Systems.</abstract><cop>Cambridge, UK</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><pmid>8006091</pmid><doi>10.1017/S0305000900008710</doi><tpages>37</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0305-0009 |
ispartof | Journal of child language, 1994-02, Vol.21 (1), p.173-209 |
issn | 0305-0009 1469-7602 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_85314147 |
source | Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); MEDLINE; Cambridge Journals; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Biological and medical sciences Child Child development Child Language Child language. Acquisition and development Child, Preschool Children Communication disorders Developmental psychology Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Grammar Homophones Humans Inflection (Grammar) Language Acquisition Language Development Linguistics Male Morphology (Languages) Nouns Phonetics Phonology Production and comprehension processes Psycholinguistics Psychology of language Psychology. Psychophysiology Reference Values Semantics Sensitivity Speech Acoustics Structural Analysis (Linguistics) Structure Verbs Young Children |
title | Sensitivity of children's inflection to grammatical structure[] |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-08T11%3A09%3A06IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sensitivity%20of%20children's%20inflection%20to%20grammatical%20structure%5B%5D&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20child%20language&rft.au=Kim,%20John%20J.&rft.date=1994-02-01&rft.volume=21&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=173&rft.epage=209&rft.pages=173-209&rft.issn=0305-0009&rft.eissn=1469-7602&rft.coden=JCLGBJ&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0305000900008710&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E76564413%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1303093474&rft_id=info:pmid/8006091&rft_cupid=10_1017_S0305000900008710&rft_ericid=EJ491174&rfr_iscdi=true |