Sensory integration and cognitive theory
Two independent but related problems are discussed: some implications of comparative psychology for learning theory, and the nature of the learning process in any given animal form. Both cognitive and stimulus-response positions in learning theory are anti-comparative because they lead to formulatio...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Psychological review 1951-09, Vol.58 (5), p.355-361 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 361 |
---|---|
container_issue | 5 |
container_start_page | 355 |
container_title | Psychological review |
container_volume | 58 |
creator | Birch, H. G Bitterman, M. E |
description | Two independent but related problems are discussed: some implications of comparative psychology for learning theory, and the nature of the learning process in any given animal form. Both cognitive and stimulus-response positions in learning theory are anti-comparative because they lead to formulations so gross as to be applicable to all animal forms. They ignore the fact that processes of modification in paramecium and rat cannot be qualitatively identical because the two animals are anatomically quite dissimilar. If we take the facts of evolution seriously, we may expect to find qualitatively distinct processes at every major level of anatomical organization. In the learning of lower mammals, at least two distinct processes must be postulated: selective motor organization and sensory integration. An imposing amount of learning data can be better understood in terms of the concept of sensory integration than of either cognitive or reinforcement theory. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1037/h0059963 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_84903865</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>84903865</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a365t-283c38145346d9bba763685199cf11d5698f2bfafc6261c6ee9f21fd77ca9efb3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp10F1LwzAUBuAgiptT8BdIUZHdVPPRpDmXMvyCgRcqeBfSNNk6unYm7WD_3oxNBMFzc24e3nN4ETon-JZglt_NMeYAgh2gIQEGKclycoiGGDOWUuCfA3QSwgLHIQDHaEAyKRnNYIjGb7YJrd8kVdPZmddd1TaJbsrEtLOm6qq1Tbq5jeAUHTldB3u23yP08fjwPnlOp69PL5P7aaqZ4F1KJTNMkoyzTJRQFDoXTEgezxpHSMkFSEcLp50RVBAjrAVHiSvz3GiwrmAjdLPLXfn2q7ehU8sqGFvXurFtH5TMADMpeISXf-Ci7X0Tf1OCZJQLSfOIrv5DhAIGmXHYqvFOGd-G4K1TK18ttd8ogtW2YPVTcKQX-8C-WNryF-4bjeB6B_RKq1XYGO27ytQ2KG_XikvFFeOcfQNs_3-e</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>614256827</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Sensory integration and cognitive theory</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>APA PsycARTICLES</source><creator>Birch, H. G ; Bitterman, M. E</creator><creatorcontrib>Birch, H. G ; Bitterman, M. E</creatorcontrib><description>Two independent but related problems are discussed: some implications of comparative psychology for learning theory, and the nature of the learning process in any given animal form. Both cognitive and stimulus-response positions in learning theory are anti-comparative because they lead to formulations so gross as to be applicable to all animal forms. They ignore the fact that processes of modification in paramecium and rat cannot be qualitatively identical because the two animals are anatomically quite dissimilar. If we take the facts of evolution seriously, we may expect to find qualitatively distinct processes at every major level of anatomical organization. In the learning of lower mammals, at least two distinct processes must be postulated: selective motor organization and sensory integration. An imposing amount of learning data can be better understood in terms of the concept of sensory integration than of either cognitive or reinforcement theory.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0033-295X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1939-1471</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1037/h0059963</identifier><identifier>PMID: 14883249</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: American Psychological Association</publisher><subject>Animal ; Animal Learning ; Cognition ; Comparative Psychology ; Humans ; Learning ; Learning Theory ; Old Medline ; Perception ; Sensory Integration</subject><ispartof>Psychological review, 1951-09, Vol.58 (5), p.355-361</ispartof><rights>1951 American Psychological Association</rights><rights>1951, American Psychological Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a365t-283c38145346d9bba763685199cf11d5698f2bfafc6261c6ee9f21fd77ca9efb3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27848,27903,27904</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14883249$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Birch, H. G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bitterman, M. E</creatorcontrib><title>Sensory integration and cognitive theory</title><title>Psychological review</title><addtitle>Psychol Rev</addtitle><description>Two independent but related problems are discussed: some implications of comparative psychology for learning theory, and the nature of the learning process in any given animal form. Both cognitive and stimulus-response positions in learning theory are anti-comparative because they lead to formulations so gross as to be applicable to all animal forms. They ignore the fact that processes of modification in paramecium and rat cannot be qualitatively identical because the two animals are anatomically quite dissimilar. If we take the facts of evolution seriously, we may expect to find qualitatively distinct processes at every major level of anatomical organization. In the learning of lower mammals, at least two distinct processes must be postulated: selective motor organization and sensory integration. An imposing amount of learning data can be better understood in terms of the concept of sensory integration than of either cognitive or reinforcement theory.</description><subject>Animal</subject><subject>Animal Learning</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Comparative Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Learning</subject><subject>Learning Theory</subject><subject>Old Medline</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Sensory Integration</subject><issn>0033-295X</issn><issn>1939-1471</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1951</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp10F1LwzAUBuAgiptT8BdIUZHdVPPRpDmXMvyCgRcqeBfSNNk6unYm7WD_3oxNBMFzc24e3nN4ETon-JZglt_NMeYAgh2gIQEGKclycoiGGDOWUuCfA3QSwgLHIQDHaEAyKRnNYIjGb7YJrd8kVdPZmddd1TaJbsrEtLOm6qq1Tbq5jeAUHTldB3u23yP08fjwPnlOp69PL5P7aaqZ4F1KJTNMkoyzTJRQFDoXTEgezxpHSMkFSEcLp50RVBAjrAVHiSvz3GiwrmAjdLPLXfn2q7ehU8sqGFvXurFtH5TMADMpeISXf-Ci7X0Tf1OCZJQLSfOIrv5DhAIGmXHYqvFOGd-G4K1TK18ttd8ogtW2YPVTcKQX-8C-WNryF-4bjeB6B_RKq1XYGO27ytQ2KG_XikvFFeOcfQNs_3-e</recordid><startdate>195109</startdate><enddate>195109</enddate><creator>Birch, H. G</creator><creator>Bitterman, M. E</creator><general>American Psychological Association</general><general>American Psychological Association, etc</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>EOLOZ</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7RZ</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>195109</creationdate><title>Sensory integration and cognitive theory</title><author>Birch, H. G ; Bitterman, M. E</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a365t-283c38145346d9bba763685199cf11d5698f2bfafc6261c6ee9f21fd77ca9efb3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1951</creationdate><topic>Animal</topic><topic>Animal Learning</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Comparative Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Learning</topic><topic>Learning Theory</topic><topic>Old Medline</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Sensory Integration</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Birch, H. G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bitterman, M. E</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 01</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>APA PsycArticles®</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Psychological review</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Birch, H. G</au><au>Bitterman, M. E</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Sensory integration and cognitive theory</atitle><jtitle>Psychological review</jtitle><addtitle>Psychol Rev</addtitle><date>1951-09</date><risdate>1951</risdate><volume>58</volume><issue>5</issue><spage>355</spage><epage>361</epage><pages>355-361</pages><issn>0033-295X</issn><eissn>1939-1471</eissn><abstract>Two independent but related problems are discussed: some implications of comparative psychology for learning theory, and the nature of the learning process in any given animal form. Both cognitive and stimulus-response positions in learning theory are anti-comparative because they lead to formulations so gross as to be applicable to all animal forms. They ignore the fact that processes of modification in paramecium and rat cannot be qualitatively identical because the two animals are anatomically quite dissimilar. If we take the facts of evolution seriously, we may expect to find qualitatively distinct processes at every major level of anatomical organization. In the learning of lower mammals, at least two distinct processes must be postulated: selective motor organization and sensory integration. An imposing amount of learning data can be better understood in terms of the concept of sensory integration than of either cognitive or reinforcement theory.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>American Psychological Association</pub><pmid>14883249</pmid><doi>10.1037/h0059963</doi><tpages>7</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0033-295X |
ispartof | Psychological review, 1951-09, Vol.58 (5), p.355-361 |
issn | 0033-295X 1939-1471 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_84903865 |
source | MEDLINE; Periodicals Index Online; APA PsycARTICLES |
subjects | Animal Animal Learning Cognition Comparative Psychology Humans Learning Learning Theory Old Medline Perception Sensory Integration |
title | Sensory integration and cognitive theory |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-25T15%3A49%3A06IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sensory%20integration%20and%20cognitive%20theory&rft.jtitle=Psychological%20review&rft.au=Birch,%20H.%20G&rft.date=1951-09&rft.volume=58&rft.issue=5&rft.spage=355&rft.epage=361&rft.pages=355-361&rft.issn=0033-295X&rft.eissn=1939-1471&rft_id=info:doi/10.1037/h0059963&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E84903865%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=614256827&rft_id=info:pmid/14883249&rfr_iscdi=true |