Effects on Sustained Performance of 48 Hours of Continuous Work and Sleep Loss

The work efficiency of 10 subjects during a 48-hr period of continuous work and sleep loss was assessed using the synthetic-work technique. Performance during the period of stress was found to be significantly influenced by the circadian rhythm. Decrements first occurred after approximately 18 hr of...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Human factors 1974-08, Vol.16 (4), p.406-414
Hauptverfasser: Morgan, Ben B., Brown, Bill R., Alluisi, Earl A.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 414
container_issue 4
container_start_page 406
container_title Human factors
container_volume 16
creator Morgan, Ben B.
Brown, Bill R.
Alluisi, Earl A.
description The work efficiency of 10 subjects during a 48-hr period of continuous work and sleep loss was assessed using the synthetic-work technique. Performance during the period of stress was found to be significantly influenced by the circadian rhythm. Decrements first occurred after approximately 18 hr of continuous work, and performance decreased to an average of 82% of baseline during the early morning hours of the first night. Performance improved to about 90% of baseline during the daytime of the second day but decreased to approximately 67% during that night. All measures of performance recovered to baseline levels following a 24-hr period of rest and recovery.
doi_str_mv 10.1177/001872087401600408
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_82591615</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sage_id>10.1177_001872087401600408</sage_id><sourcerecordid>1311842964</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-ecf78bcb919028762f30f8f81d472d086364cf7adf3b239fc775ecf5592e9443</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kV9LwzAUxYMoc_75AoIQEHyru0mTJnmUMZ0wVNjAx9KmiXR2zUzaB7-9KRsiiublBu7vnHsuF6ELAjeECDEBIFJQkIIByQAYyAM0JpyJRBJJDtF4AJKBOEYnIawBIFMpH6ERYykXCsbocWat0V3ArsXLPnRF3ZoKPxtvnd8UrTbYWcwknrveh-E_dW1Xt73rA35x_g0XbYWXjTFbvHAhnKEjWzTBnO_rKVrdzVbTebJ4un-Y3i4SnSrWJUZbIUtdKqKASpFRm4KVVpKKCVqBzNKMRaSobFrSVFktBI8azhU1KmY_Rdc72613770JXb6pgzZNU7QmJssl5YpkhEfw6ge4jou0MVpOlODxKYB_qZQQyajKhqF0R2kfN_XG5ltfbwr_kRPIh3vkv-8RRZd7677cmOpLsj9A7E92_VC8mm9j_3b8BBIpj3k</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1311842964</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Effects on Sustained Performance of 48 Hours of Continuous Work and Sleep Loss</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SAGE Complete</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Morgan, Ben B. ; Brown, Bill R. ; Alluisi, Earl A.</creator><creatorcontrib>Morgan, Ben B. ; Brown, Bill R. ; Alluisi, Earl A.</creatorcontrib><description>The work efficiency of 10 subjects during a 48-hr period of continuous work and sleep loss was assessed using the synthetic-work technique. Performance during the period of stress was found to be significantly influenced by the circadian rhythm. Decrements first occurred after approximately 18 hr of continuous work, and performance decreased to an average of 82% of baseline during the early morning hours of the first night. Performance improved to about 90% of baseline during the daytime of the second day but decreased to approximately 67% during that night. All measures of performance recovered to baseline levels following a 24-hr period of rest and recovery.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0018-7208</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1547-8181</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1177/001872087401600408</identifier><identifier>PMID: 4435790</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Attention ; Circadian Rhythm ; Circadian rhythms ; Efficiency ; Humans ; Male ; Memory ; Night ; Perception ; Problem Solving ; Rest ; Sleep ; Sleep Deprivation ; Space life sciences ; Task Performance and Analysis ; Time Factors ; Work</subject><ispartof>Human factors, 1974-08, Vol.16 (4), p.406-414</ispartof><rights>1974 Human Factors and Ergonomics Society</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-ecf78bcb919028762f30f8f81d472d086364cf7adf3b239fc775ecf5592e9443</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/001872087401600408$$EPDF$$P50$$Gsage$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/001872087401600408$$EHTML$$P50$$Gsage$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,21798,27846,27901,27902,43597,43598</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4435790$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Morgan, Ben B.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brown, Bill R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alluisi, Earl A.</creatorcontrib><title>Effects on Sustained Performance of 48 Hours of Continuous Work and Sleep Loss</title><title>Human factors</title><addtitle>Hum Factors</addtitle><description>The work efficiency of 10 subjects during a 48-hr period of continuous work and sleep loss was assessed using the synthetic-work technique. Performance during the period of stress was found to be significantly influenced by the circadian rhythm. Decrements first occurred after approximately 18 hr of continuous work, and performance decreased to an average of 82% of baseline during the early morning hours of the first night. Performance improved to about 90% of baseline during the daytime of the second day but decreased to approximately 67% during that night. All measures of performance recovered to baseline levels following a 24-hr period of rest and recovery.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Circadian Rhythm</subject><subject>Circadian rhythms</subject><subject>Efficiency</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Memory</subject><subject>Night</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Problem Solving</subject><subject>Rest</subject><subject>Sleep</subject><subject>Sleep Deprivation</subject><subject>Space life sciences</subject><subject>Task Performance and Analysis</subject><subject>Time Factors</subject><subject>Work</subject><issn>0018-7208</issn><issn>1547-8181</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1974</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kV9LwzAUxYMoc_75AoIQEHyru0mTJnmUMZ0wVNjAx9KmiXR2zUzaB7-9KRsiiublBu7vnHsuF6ELAjeECDEBIFJQkIIByQAYyAM0JpyJRBJJDtF4AJKBOEYnIawBIFMpH6ERYykXCsbocWat0V3ArsXLPnRF3ZoKPxtvnd8UrTbYWcwknrveh-E_dW1Xt73rA35x_g0XbYWXjTFbvHAhnKEjWzTBnO_rKVrdzVbTebJ4un-Y3i4SnSrWJUZbIUtdKqKASpFRm4KVVpKKCVqBzNKMRaSobFrSVFktBI8azhU1KmY_Rdc72613770JXb6pgzZNU7QmJssl5YpkhEfw6ge4jou0MVpOlODxKYB_qZQQyajKhqF0R2kfN_XG5ltfbwr_kRPIh3vkv-8RRZd7677cmOpLsj9A7E92_VC8mm9j_3b8BBIpj3k</recordid><startdate>197408</startdate><enddate>197408</enddate><creator>Morgan, Ben B.</creator><creator>Brown, Bill R.</creator><creator>Alluisi, Earl A.</creator><general>SAGE Publications</general><general>Human Factors Society of America</general><general>Human Factors and Ergonomics Society</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>JRZRW</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7QF</scope><scope>7QQ</scope><scope>7SC</scope><scope>7SE</scope><scope>7SP</scope><scope>7SR</scope><scope>7T2</scope><scope>7TA</scope><scope>7TB</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7U5</scope><scope>8BQ</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>F28</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>H8D</scope><scope>H8G</scope><scope>JG9</scope><scope>JQ2</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KR7</scope><scope>L7M</scope><scope>L~C</scope><scope>L~D</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>197408</creationdate><title>Effects on Sustained Performance of 48 Hours of Continuous Work and Sleep Loss</title><author>Morgan, Ben B. ; Brown, Bill R. ; Alluisi, Earl A.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c394t-ecf78bcb919028762f30f8f81d472d086364cf7adf3b239fc775ecf5592e9443</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1974</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Circadian Rhythm</topic><topic>Circadian rhythms</topic><topic>Efficiency</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Memory</topic><topic>Night</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Problem Solving</topic><topic>Rest</topic><topic>Sleep</topic><topic>Sleep Deprivation</topic><topic>Space life sciences</topic><topic>Task Performance and Analysis</topic><topic>Time Factors</topic><topic>Work</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Morgan, Ben B.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Brown, Bill R.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Alluisi, Earl A.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 35</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Aluminium Industry Abstracts</collection><collection>Ceramic Abstracts</collection><collection>Computer and Information Systems Abstracts</collection><collection>Corrosion Abstracts</collection><collection>Electronics &amp; Communications Abstracts</collection><collection>Engineered Materials Abstracts</collection><collection>Health and Safety Science Abstracts (Full archive)</collection><collection>Materials Business File</collection><collection>Mechanical &amp; Transportation Engineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Solid State and Superconductivity Abstracts</collection><collection>METADEX</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ANTE: Abstracts in New Technology &amp; Engineering</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Aerospace Database</collection><collection>Copper Technical Reference Library</collection><collection>Materials Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Computer Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Civil Engineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Advanced Technologies Database with Aerospace</collection><collection>Computer and Information Systems Abstracts – Academic</collection><collection>Computer and Information Systems Abstracts Professional</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Human factors</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Morgan, Ben B.</au><au>Brown, Bill R.</au><au>Alluisi, Earl A.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Effects on Sustained Performance of 48 Hours of Continuous Work and Sleep Loss</atitle><jtitle>Human factors</jtitle><addtitle>Hum Factors</addtitle><date>1974-08</date><risdate>1974</risdate><volume>16</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>406</spage><epage>414</epage><pages>406-414</pages><issn>0018-7208</issn><eissn>1547-8181</eissn><abstract>The work efficiency of 10 subjects during a 48-hr period of continuous work and sleep loss was assessed using the synthetic-work technique. Performance during the period of stress was found to be significantly influenced by the circadian rhythm. Decrements first occurred after approximately 18 hr of continuous work, and performance decreased to an average of 82% of baseline during the early morning hours of the first night. Performance improved to about 90% of baseline during the daytime of the second day but decreased to approximately 67% during that night. All measures of performance recovered to baseline levels following a 24-hr period of rest and recovery.</abstract><cop>Los Angeles, CA</cop><pub>SAGE Publications</pub><pmid>4435790</pmid><doi>10.1177/001872087401600408</doi><tpages>9</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0018-7208
ispartof Human factors, 1974-08, Vol.16 (4), p.406-414
issn 0018-7208
1547-8181
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_82591615
source MEDLINE; SAGE Complete; Periodicals Index Online
subjects Adolescent
Adult
Attention
Circadian Rhythm
Circadian rhythms
Efficiency
Humans
Male
Memory
Night
Perception
Problem Solving
Rest
Sleep
Sleep Deprivation
Space life sciences
Task Performance and Analysis
Time Factors
Work
title Effects on Sustained Performance of 48 Hours of Continuous Work and Sleep Loss
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-12T23%3A27%3A41IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Effects%20on%20Sustained%20Performance%20of%2048%20Hours%20of%20Continuous%20Work%20and%20Sleep%20Loss&rft.jtitle=Human%20factors&rft.au=Morgan,%20Ben%20B.&rft.date=1974-08&rft.volume=16&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=406&rft.epage=414&rft.pages=406-414&rft.issn=0018-7208&rft.eissn=1547-8181&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177/001872087401600408&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1311842964%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1311842964&rft_id=info:pmid/4435790&rft_sage_id=10.1177_001872087401600408&rfr_iscdi=true