Infants' Perception of Melodies: The Role of Melodic Contour
Infants 8-11 months of age were exposed to repetitions of a 6-tone sequence or melody, then tested for their discrimination of transpositions of that sequence as well as other melodic transformations previously used by Massaro, Kallman, and Kelly with adults. In Experiment 1, infants showed evidence...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Child development 1984-06, Vol.55 (3), p.821-830 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 830 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 821 |
container_title | Child development |
container_volume | 55 |
creator | Trehub, Sandra E. Bull, Dale Thorpe, Leigh A. |
description | Infants 8-11 months of age were exposed to repetitions of a 6-tone sequence or melody, then tested for their discrimination of transpositions of that sequence as well as other melodic transformations previously used by Massaro, Kallman, and Kelly with adults. In Experiment 1, infants showed evidence of discriminating all transformations from the original melody. In Experiment 2, the task was made more difficult, and infants failed to discriminate transpositions of the original melody as well as transformations that preserved the melodic contour and approximate frequency range of the original melody. By contrast, infants showed evidence of discriminating transformations that violated the contour of the original melody or that included changes in the octaves from which component tones were drawn. This global processing strategy parallels that used by adults with atonal or unfamiliar tonal melodies. |
doi_str_mv | 10.2307/1130133 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_81113595</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><jstor_id>1130133</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>1130133</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c294t-be2d52c7ca041429722d9a85f907906375672d9333c51ef91b4d5d8b666d01d83</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kFtLw0AQhRdRaq3iLxACin2K7v0ivkjxUqgoUp_DZneDKWm27iYP_ntXGiwIPg1z5uPMzAHgFMErTKC4RohARMgeGCPKRS45pvtgDCFUOVEYHoKjGFepxVyRERhxQSjBcAxu522l2y5Os1cXjNt0tW8zX2XPrvG2dvEmW3647M03bqeabObbzvfhGBxUuonuZKgT8P5wv5w95YuXx_nsbpEbrGiXlw5bho0wGlJEsRIYW6UlqxQUCnIiGBdJIYQYhlylUEkts7LknFuIrCQTcLn13QT_2bvYFes6Gtc0unW-j4VE6X2mWALP_4CrdGabbisQVhxLzDFP1HRLmeBjDK4qNqFe6_BVIFj8pFkMaSbybPDry7Wzv9wQX5pfDHMdjW6qoFtTx19MSioVZTtsFTsf_t32DZlOgkw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1296282626</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Infants' Perception of Melodies: The Role of Melodic Contour</title><source>Jstor Complete Legacy</source><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Education Source</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Trehub, Sandra E. ; Bull, Dale ; Thorpe, Leigh A.</creator><creatorcontrib>Trehub, Sandra E. ; Bull, Dale ; Thorpe, Leigh A.</creatorcontrib><description>Infants 8-11 months of age were exposed to repetitions of a 6-tone sequence or melody, then tested for their discrimination of transpositions of that sequence as well as other melodic transformations previously used by Massaro, Kallman, and Kelly with adults. In Experiment 1, infants showed evidence of discriminating all transformations from the original melody. In Experiment 2, the task was made more difficult, and infants failed to discriminate transpositions of the original melody as well as transformations that preserved the melodic contour and approximate frequency range of the original melody. By contrast, infants showed evidence of discriminating transformations that violated the contour of the original melody or that included changes in the octaves from which component tones were drawn. This global processing strategy parallels that used by adults with atonal or unfamiliar tonal melodies.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0009-3920</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-8624</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.2307/1130133</identifier><identifier>PMID: 6734320</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CHDEAW</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Malden, MA: University of Chicago Press</publisher><subject>Attention ; Auditory perception ; Biological and medical sciences ; Child development ; Developmental psychology ; Experimentation ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Infant ; Infants ; Male ; Melodic contours ; Melody ; Music ; Musical intervals ; Musical perception ; Octaves ; Pitch Discrimination ; Pitch Perception ; Psychology, Child ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Row transformations ; Tonal sequences</subject><ispartof>Child development, 1984-06, Vol.55 (3), p.821-830</ispartof><rights>Copyright 1984 The Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.</rights><rights>1985 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c294t-be2d52c7ca041429722d9a85f907906375672d9333c51ef91b4d5d8b666d01d83</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/1130133$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/1130133$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,799,27846,27901,27902,57992,58225</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=8848945$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6734320$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Trehub, Sandra E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bull, Dale</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Thorpe, Leigh A.</creatorcontrib><title>Infants' Perception of Melodies: The Role of Melodic Contour</title><title>Child development</title><addtitle>Child Dev</addtitle><description>Infants 8-11 months of age were exposed to repetitions of a 6-tone sequence or melody, then tested for their discrimination of transpositions of that sequence as well as other melodic transformations previously used by Massaro, Kallman, and Kelly with adults. In Experiment 1, infants showed evidence of discriminating all transformations from the original melody. In Experiment 2, the task was made more difficult, and infants failed to discriminate transpositions of the original melody as well as transformations that preserved the melodic contour and approximate frequency range of the original melody. By contrast, infants showed evidence of discriminating transformations that violated the contour of the original melody or that included changes in the octaves from which component tones were drawn. This global processing strategy parallels that used by adults with atonal or unfamiliar tonal melodies.</description><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Auditory perception</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Experimentation</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infant</subject><subject>Infants</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Melodic contours</subject><subject>Melody</subject><subject>Music</subject><subject>Musical intervals</subject><subject>Musical perception</subject><subject>Octaves</subject><subject>Pitch Discrimination</subject><subject>Pitch Perception</subject><subject>Psychology, Child</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Row transformations</subject><subject>Tonal sequences</subject><issn>0009-3920</issn><issn>1467-8624</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1984</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kFtLw0AQhRdRaq3iLxACin2K7v0ivkjxUqgoUp_DZneDKWm27iYP_ntXGiwIPg1z5uPMzAHgFMErTKC4RohARMgeGCPKRS45pvtgDCFUOVEYHoKjGFepxVyRERhxQSjBcAxu522l2y5Os1cXjNt0tW8zX2XPrvG2dvEmW3647M03bqeabObbzvfhGBxUuonuZKgT8P5wv5w95YuXx_nsbpEbrGiXlw5bho0wGlJEsRIYW6UlqxQUCnIiGBdJIYQYhlylUEkts7LknFuIrCQTcLn13QT_2bvYFes6Gtc0unW-j4VE6X2mWALP_4CrdGabbisQVhxLzDFP1HRLmeBjDK4qNqFe6_BVIFj8pFkMaSbybPDry7Wzv9wQX5pfDHMdjW6qoFtTx19MSioVZTtsFTsf_t32DZlOgkw</recordid><startdate>198406</startdate><enddate>198406</enddate><creator>Trehub, Sandra E.</creator><creator>Bull, Dale</creator><creator>Thorpe, Leigh A.</creator><general>University of Chicago Press</general><general>Blackwell</general><general>University of Chicago Press for the Society for Research in Child Development, etc</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>FIXVA</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>198406</creationdate><title>Infants' Perception of Melodies: The Role of Melodic Contour</title><author>Trehub, Sandra E. ; Bull, Dale ; Thorpe, Leigh A.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c294t-be2d52c7ca041429722d9a85f907906375672d9333c51ef91b4d5d8b666d01d83</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1984</creationdate><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Auditory perception</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Experimentation</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infant</topic><topic>Infants</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Melodic contours</topic><topic>Melody</topic><topic>Music</topic><topic>Musical intervals</topic><topic>Musical perception</topic><topic>Octaves</topic><topic>Pitch Discrimination</topic><topic>Pitch Perception</topic><topic>Psychology, Child</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Row transformations</topic><topic>Tonal sequences</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Trehub, Sandra E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bull, Dale</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Thorpe, Leigh A.</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 03</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Child development</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Trehub, Sandra E.</au><au>Bull, Dale</au><au>Thorpe, Leigh A.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Infants' Perception of Melodies: The Role of Melodic Contour</atitle><jtitle>Child development</jtitle><addtitle>Child Dev</addtitle><date>1984-06</date><risdate>1984</risdate><volume>55</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>821</spage><epage>830</epage><pages>821-830</pages><issn>0009-3920</issn><eissn>1467-8624</eissn><coden>CHDEAW</coden><abstract>Infants 8-11 months of age were exposed to repetitions of a 6-tone sequence or melody, then tested for their discrimination of transpositions of that sequence as well as other melodic transformations previously used by Massaro, Kallman, and Kelly with adults. In Experiment 1, infants showed evidence of discriminating all transformations from the original melody. In Experiment 2, the task was made more difficult, and infants failed to discriminate transpositions of the original melody as well as transformations that preserved the melodic contour and approximate frequency range of the original melody. By contrast, infants showed evidence of discriminating transformations that violated the contour of the original melody or that included changes in the octaves from which component tones were drawn. This global processing strategy parallels that used by adults with atonal or unfamiliar tonal melodies.</abstract><cop>Malden, MA</cop><pub>University of Chicago Press</pub><pmid>6734320</pmid><doi>10.2307/1130133</doi><tpages>10</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0009-3920 |
ispartof | Child development, 1984-06, Vol.55 (3), p.821-830 |
issn | 0009-3920 1467-8624 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_81113595 |
source | Jstor Complete Legacy; MEDLINE; Education Source; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Attention Auditory perception Biological and medical sciences Child development Developmental psychology Experimentation Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Infant Infants Male Melodic contours Melody Music Musical intervals Musical perception Octaves Pitch Discrimination Pitch Perception Psychology, Child Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Row transformations Tonal sequences |
title | Infants' Perception of Melodies: The Role of Melodic Contour |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-03T16%3A09%3A44IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Infants'%20Perception%20of%20Melodies:%20The%20Role%20of%20Melodic%20Contour&rft.jtitle=Child%20development&rft.au=Trehub,%20Sandra%20E.&rft.date=1984-06&rft.volume=55&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=821&rft.epage=830&rft.pages=821-830&rft.issn=0009-3920&rft.eissn=1467-8624&rft.coden=CHDEAW&rft_id=info:doi/10.2307/1130133&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E1130133%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1296282626&rft_id=info:pmid/6734320&rft_jstor_id=1130133&rfr_iscdi=true |