Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition
To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level – mor...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | The Behavioral and brain sciences 1997-12, Vol.20 (4), p.723-742 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 742 |
---|---|
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | 723 |
container_title | The Behavioral and brain sciences |
container_volume | 20 |
creator | Ballard, Dana H. Hayhoe, Mary M. Pook, Polly K. Rao, Rajesh P. N. |
description | To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales,
computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels
of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second,
orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and
form a useful computational level – more abstract than that
used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is
traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as
reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints
of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations.
The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the
natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural
computational economies of sequential decision systems through a
system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing
movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs.
This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible
to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism
for representing the essential features that link external sensory
data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the
central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to
moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye
movements and hand movements. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1017/S0140525X97001611 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_79643159</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S0140525X97001611</cupid><sourcerecordid>1877063436</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-9cafd3488823fc8320951c3d3f7fe2b390fded44d1b2851321f742b0ac7545af3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kEtrFEEUhYsQccbRH5BNaCK4a723Hl1VS_MwEQISohKyKbrrEStOd8WqHtB_bzczhKBkdRfnO5ePQ8gBwnsElB-uATkIKm60BMAGcY8skTe6RkXFPlnOcT3nC_KqlHsAEFzol2SBAHNFL0l96qMdo61scr5UIeVq_OEr33fJxd4PY5XClN0NcYxpeE1ehHZd_JvdXZFvn86-nlzUl1_OP598vKytABhrbdvgGFdKURasYhS0QMscCzJ42jENwXnHucOOKoGMYpCcdtBaOQm2ga3Iu-3fh5x-bXwZTR-L9et1O_i0KUbqhjMUegKP_gHv0yYPk5uhSCk2Ss3Q2-cgVFJCwzhrJgq3lM2plOyDecixb_Mfg2Dmuc1_c0-dw93nTdd796Sx3XcC6i0Qy-h_P-Zt_mkayaQwzfmVoTeMfr89Zmbm2U6i7bsc3Z1_4vqsxl9d-pV3</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877063436</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Cambridge Journals</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Ballard, Dana H. ; Hayhoe, Mary M. ; Pook, Polly K. ; Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creator><creatorcontrib>Ballard, Dana H. ; Hayhoe, Mary M. ; Pook, Polly K. ; Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creatorcontrib><description>To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales,
computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels
of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second,
orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and
form a useful computational level – more abstract than that
used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is
traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as
reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints
of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations.
The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the
natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural
computational economies of sequential decision systems through a
system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing
movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs.
This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible
to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism
for representing the essential features that link external sensory
data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the
central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to
moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye
movements and hand movements.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0140-525X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-1825</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X97001611</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10097009</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BBSCDH</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>binding ; Brain ; Brain - physiology ; brain computation ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognition - physiology ; deictic computations ; embodiment ; eye movements ; Eye Movements - physiology ; Humans ; Language ; Memory ; Memory - physiology ; Motor ability ; natural tasks ; Neurology ; Neurons ; pointers ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Senses ; sensory-motor tasks ; working memory</subject><ispartof>The Behavioral and brain sciences, 1997-12, Vol.20 (4), p.723-742</ispartof><rights>1997 Cambridge University Press</rights><rights>Copyright Cambridge University Press, Publishing Division Dec 1997</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-9cafd3488823fc8320951c3d3f7fe2b390fded44d1b2851321f742b0ac7545af3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0140525X97001611/type/journal_article$$EHTML$$P50$$Gcambridge$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>164,314,780,784,27869,27924,27925,55628</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10097009$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Ballard, Dana H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hayhoe, Mary M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pook, Polly K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creatorcontrib><title>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</title><title>The Behavioral and brain sciences</title><addtitle>Behav Brain Sci</addtitle><description>To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales,
computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels
of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second,
orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and
form a useful computational level – more abstract than that
used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is
traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as
reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints
of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations.
The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the
natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural
computational economies of sequential decision systems through a
system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing
movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs.
This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible
to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism
for representing the essential features that link external sensory
data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the
central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to
moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye
movements and hand movements.</description><subject>binding</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>brain computation</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>deictic computations</subject><subject>embodiment</subject><subject>eye movements</subject><subject>Eye Movements - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Memory</subject><subject>Memory - physiology</subject><subject>Motor ability</subject><subject>natural tasks</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurons</subject><subject>pointers</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Senses</subject><subject>sensory-motor tasks</subject><subject>working memory</subject><issn>0140-525X</issn><issn>1469-1825</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1997</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kEtrFEEUhYsQccbRH5BNaCK4a723Hl1VS_MwEQISohKyKbrrEStOd8WqHtB_bzczhKBkdRfnO5ePQ8gBwnsElB-uATkIKm60BMAGcY8skTe6RkXFPlnOcT3nC_KqlHsAEFzol2SBAHNFL0l96qMdo61scr5UIeVq_OEr33fJxd4PY5XClN0NcYxpeE1ehHZd_JvdXZFvn86-nlzUl1_OP598vKytABhrbdvgGFdKURasYhS0QMscCzJ42jENwXnHucOOKoGMYpCcdtBaOQm2ga3Iu-3fh5x-bXwZTR-L9et1O_i0KUbqhjMUegKP_gHv0yYPk5uhSCk2Ss3Q2-cgVFJCwzhrJgq3lM2plOyDecixb_Mfg2Dmuc1_c0-dw93nTdd796Sx3XcC6i0Qy-h_P-Zt_mkayaQwzfmVoTeMfr89Zmbm2U6i7bsc3Z1_4vqsxl9d-pV3</recordid><startdate>19971201</startdate><enddate>19971201</enddate><creator>Ballard, Dana H.</creator><creator>Hayhoe, Mary M.</creator><creator>Pook, Polly K.</creator><creator>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>HJHVS</scope><scope>IBDFT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19971201</creationdate><title>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</title><author>Ballard, Dana H. ; Hayhoe, Mary M. ; Pook, Polly K. ; Rao, Rajesh P. N.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-9cafd3488823fc8320951c3d3f7fe2b390fded44d1b2851321f742b0ac7545af3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1997</creationdate><topic>binding</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>brain computation</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>deictic computations</topic><topic>embodiment</topic><topic>eye movements</topic><topic>Eye Movements - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Memory</topic><topic>Memory - physiology</topic><topic>Motor ability</topic><topic>natural tasks</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurons</topic><topic>pointers</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Senses</topic><topic>sensory-motor tasks</topic><topic>working memory</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ballard, Dana H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hayhoe, Mary M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pook, Polly K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 19</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 27</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>The Behavioral and brain sciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ballard, Dana H.</au><au>Hayhoe, Mary M.</au><au>Pook, Polly K.</au><au>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</atitle><jtitle>The Behavioral and brain sciences</jtitle><addtitle>Behav Brain Sci</addtitle><date>1997-12-01</date><risdate>1997</risdate><volume>20</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>723</spage><epage>742</epage><pages>723-742</pages><issn>0140-525X</issn><eissn>1469-1825</eissn><coden>BBSCDH</coden><abstract>To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales,
computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels
of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second,
orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and
form a useful computational level – more abstract than that
used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is
traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as
reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints
of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations.
The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the
natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural
computational economies of sequential decision systems through a
system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing
movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs.
This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible
to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism
for representing the essential features that link external sensory
data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the
central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to
moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye
movements and hand movements.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><pmid>10097009</pmid><doi>10.1017/S0140525X97001611</doi><tpages>20</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0140-525X |
ispartof | The Behavioral and brain sciences, 1997-12, Vol.20 (4), p.723-742 |
issn | 0140-525X 1469-1825 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_79643159 |
source | MEDLINE; Cambridge Journals; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | binding Brain Brain - physiology brain computation Cognition & reasoning Cognition - physiology deictic computations embodiment eye movements Eye Movements - physiology Humans Language Memory Memory - physiology Motor ability natural tasks Neurology Neurons pointers Psychomotor Performance - physiology Senses sensory-motor tasks working memory |
title | Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-05T04%3A14%3A41IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Deictic%20codes%20for%20the%20embodiment%20of%20cognition&rft.jtitle=The%20Behavioral%20and%20brain%20sciences&rft.au=Ballard,%20Dana%20H.&rft.date=1997-12-01&rft.volume=20&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=723&rft.epage=742&rft.pages=723-742&rft.issn=0140-525X&rft.eissn=1469-1825&rft.coden=BBSCDH&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0140525X97001611&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1877063436%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877063436&rft_id=info:pmid/10097009&rft_cupid=10_1017_S0140525X97001611&rfr_iscdi=true |