Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition

To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level – mor...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:The Behavioral and brain sciences 1997-12, Vol.20 (4), p.723-742
Hauptverfasser: Ballard, Dana H., Hayhoe, Mary M., Pook, Polly K., Rao, Rajesh P. N.
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 742
container_issue 4
container_start_page 723
container_title The Behavioral and brain sciences
container_volume 20
creator Ballard, Dana H.
Hayhoe, Mary M.
Pook, Polly K.
Rao, Rajesh P. N.
description To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level – more abstract than that used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations. The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural computational economies of sequential decision systems through a system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs. This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism for representing the essential features that link external sensory data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye movements and hand movements.
doi_str_mv 10.1017/S0140525X97001611
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_79643159</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S0140525X97001611</cupid><sourcerecordid>1877063436</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-9cafd3488823fc8320951c3d3f7fe2b390fded44d1b2851321f742b0ac7545af3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kEtrFEEUhYsQccbRH5BNaCK4a723Hl1VS_MwEQISohKyKbrrEStOd8WqHtB_bzczhKBkdRfnO5ePQ8gBwnsElB-uATkIKm60BMAGcY8skTe6RkXFPlnOcT3nC_KqlHsAEFzol2SBAHNFL0l96qMdo61scr5UIeVq_OEr33fJxd4PY5XClN0NcYxpeE1ehHZd_JvdXZFvn86-nlzUl1_OP598vKytABhrbdvgGFdKURasYhS0QMscCzJ42jENwXnHucOOKoGMYpCcdtBaOQm2ga3Iu-3fh5x-bXwZTR-L9et1O_i0KUbqhjMUegKP_gHv0yYPk5uhSCk2Ss3Q2-cgVFJCwzhrJgq3lM2plOyDecixb_Mfg2Dmuc1_c0-dw93nTdd796Sx3XcC6i0Qy-h_P-Zt_mkayaQwzfmVoTeMfr89Zmbm2U6i7bsc3Z1_4vqsxl9d-pV3</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877063436</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Cambridge Journals</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Ballard, Dana H. ; Hayhoe, Mary M. ; Pook, Polly K. ; Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creator><creatorcontrib>Ballard, Dana H. ; Hayhoe, Mary M. ; Pook, Polly K. ; Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creatorcontrib><description>To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level – more abstract than that used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations. The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural computational economies of sequential decision systems through a system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs. This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism for representing the essential features that link external sensory data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye movements and hand movements.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0140-525X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-1825</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X97001611</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10097009</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BBSCDH</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>England: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>binding ; Brain ; Brain - physiology ; brain computation ; Cognition &amp; reasoning ; Cognition - physiology ; deictic computations ; embodiment ; eye movements ; Eye Movements - physiology ; Humans ; Language ; Memory ; Memory - physiology ; Motor ability ; natural tasks ; Neurology ; Neurons ; pointers ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Senses ; sensory-motor tasks ; working memory</subject><ispartof>The Behavioral and brain sciences, 1997-12, Vol.20 (4), p.723-742</ispartof><rights>1997 Cambridge University Press</rights><rights>Copyright Cambridge University Press, Publishing Division Dec 1997</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-9cafd3488823fc8320951c3d3f7fe2b390fded44d1b2851321f742b0ac7545af3</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0140525X97001611/type/journal_article$$EHTML$$P50$$Gcambridge$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>164,314,780,784,27869,27924,27925,55628</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10097009$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Ballard, Dana H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hayhoe, Mary M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pook, Polly K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creatorcontrib><title>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</title><title>The Behavioral and brain sciences</title><addtitle>Behav Brain Sci</addtitle><description>To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level – more abstract than that used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations. The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural computational economies of sequential decision systems through a system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs. This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism for representing the essential features that link external sensory data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye movements and hand movements.</description><subject>binding</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>brain computation</subject><subject>Cognition &amp; reasoning</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>deictic computations</subject><subject>embodiment</subject><subject>eye movements</subject><subject>Eye Movements - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Memory</subject><subject>Memory - physiology</subject><subject>Motor ability</subject><subject>natural tasks</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurons</subject><subject>pointers</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Senses</subject><subject>sensory-motor tasks</subject><subject>working memory</subject><issn>0140-525X</issn><issn>1469-1825</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1997</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kEtrFEEUhYsQccbRH5BNaCK4a723Hl1VS_MwEQISohKyKbrrEStOd8WqHtB_bzczhKBkdRfnO5ePQ8gBwnsElB-uATkIKm60BMAGcY8skTe6RkXFPlnOcT3nC_KqlHsAEFzol2SBAHNFL0l96qMdo61scr5UIeVq_OEr33fJxd4PY5XClN0NcYxpeE1ehHZd_JvdXZFvn86-nlzUl1_OP598vKytABhrbdvgGFdKURasYhS0QMscCzJ42jENwXnHucOOKoGMYpCcdtBaOQm2ga3Iu-3fh5x-bXwZTR-L9et1O_i0KUbqhjMUegKP_gHv0yYPk5uhSCk2Ss3Q2-cgVFJCwzhrJgq3lM2plOyDecixb_Mfg2Dmuc1_c0-dw93nTdd796Sx3XcC6i0Qy-h_P-Zt_mkayaQwzfmVoTeMfr89Zmbm2U6i7bsc3Z1_4vqsxl9d-pV3</recordid><startdate>19971201</startdate><enddate>19971201</enddate><creator>Ballard, Dana H.</creator><creator>Hayhoe, Mary M.</creator><creator>Pook, Polly K.</creator><creator>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>HJHVS</scope><scope>IBDFT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19971201</creationdate><title>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</title><author>Ballard, Dana H. ; Hayhoe, Mary M. ; Pook, Polly K. ; Rao, Rajesh P. N.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c500t-9cafd3488823fc8320951c3d3f7fe2b390fded44d1b2851321f742b0ac7545af3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1997</creationdate><topic>binding</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>brain computation</topic><topic>Cognition &amp; reasoning</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>deictic computations</topic><topic>embodiment</topic><topic>eye movements</topic><topic>Eye Movements - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Memory</topic><topic>Memory - physiology</topic><topic>Motor ability</topic><topic>natural tasks</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurons</topic><topic>pointers</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Senses</topic><topic>sensory-motor tasks</topic><topic>working memory</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Ballard, Dana H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hayhoe, Mary M.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Pook, Polly K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 19</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 27</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>The Behavioral and brain sciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Ballard, Dana H.</au><au>Hayhoe, Mary M.</au><au>Pook, Polly K.</au><au>Rao, Rajesh P. N.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition</atitle><jtitle>The Behavioral and brain sciences</jtitle><addtitle>Behav Brain Sci</addtitle><date>1997-12-01</date><risdate>1997</risdate><volume>20</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>723</spage><epage>742</epage><pages>723-742</pages><issn>0140-525X</issn><eissn>1469-1825</eissn><coden>BBSCDH</coden><abstract>To describe phenomena that occur at different time scales, computational models of the brain must incorporate different levels of abstraction. At time scales of approximately 1/3 of a second, orienting movements of the body play a crucial role in cognition and form a useful computational level – more abstract than that used to capture natural phenomena but less abstract than what is traditionally used to study high-level cognitive processes such as reasoning. At this “embodiment level,” the constraints of the physical system determine the nature of cognitive operations. The key synergy is that at time scales of about 1/3 of a second, the natural sequentiality of body movements can be matched to the natural computational economies of sequential decision systems through a system of implicit reference called deictic in which pointing movements are used to bind objects in the world to cognitive programs. This target article focuses on how deictic bindings make it possible to perform natural tasks. Deictic computation provides a mechanism for representing the essential features that link external sensory data with internal cognitive programs and motor actions. One of the central features of cognition, working memory, can be related to moment-by-moment dispositions of body features such as eye movements and hand movements.</abstract><cop>England</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><pmid>10097009</pmid><doi>10.1017/S0140525X97001611</doi><tpages>20</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0140-525X
ispartof The Behavioral and brain sciences, 1997-12, Vol.20 (4), p.723-742
issn 0140-525X
1469-1825
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_79643159
source MEDLINE; Cambridge Journals; Periodicals Index Online
subjects binding
Brain
Brain - physiology
brain computation
Cognition & reasoning
Cognition - physiology
deictic computations
embodiment
eye movements
Eye Movements - physiology
Humans
Language
Memory
Memory - physiology
Motor ability
natural tasks
Neurology
Neurons
pointers
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Senses
sensory-motor tasks
working memory
title Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-05T04%3A14%3A41IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Deictic%20codes%20for%20the%20embodiment%20of%20cognition&rft.jtitle=The%20Behavioral%20and%20brain%20sciences&rft.au=Ballard,%20Dana%20H.&rft.date=1997-12-01&rft.volume=20&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=723&rft.epage=742&rft.pages=723-742&rft.issn=0140-525X&rft.eissn=1469-1825&rft.coden=BBSCDH&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0140525X97001611&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1877063436%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877063436&rft_id=info:pmid/10097009&rft_cupid=10_1017_S0140525X97001611&rfr_iscdi=true