Age-Related Dynamics of Cognitive Brain Functions in Humans: An Electrophysiological Approach
Event-related P300 potentials closely reflect cognitive functions such as stimulus discrimination (N250) and processing time (P300 latencies) as well as attention capabilities (P300 amplitudes). To delineate the age-related dynamics of P300 potentials, we investigated 250 healthy subjects between 18...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences Biological sciences and medical sciences, 1996-01, Vol.51A (1), p.B3-B16 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | B16 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | B3 |
container_title | The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences |
container_volume | 51A |
creator | Kügler, Christian F. A. Petter, Jürgen Platt, Dieter |
description | Event-related P300 potentials closely reflect cognitive functions such as stimulus discrimination (N250) and processing time (P300 latencies) as well as attention capabilities (P300 amplitudes). To delineate the age-related dynamics of P300 potentials, we investigated 250 healthy subjects between 18 and 98 years of age in a cross-sectional study. A total of 330 visual P300 tests was performed in two different paradigms (PI, passive condition, n = 80; PII, active condition, n = 250). In both P300 paradigms, the N250 and P300 latencies were markedly prolonged (p < .0001) in older age, whereas the N250 and P300 latency differences between PII and PI did not change (p > .05). The P300 amplitudes in paradigm I and II revealed only a slight age-related reduction. In fact, the P300 amplitude ratios between PII and PI remained constant. Third-order polynomial regressions provided the best fit of the aging-P300 interactions in paradigms I and II for both males and females. Interestingly, females showed a greater and possibly earlier P300 latency increase during aging than males. These age-related changes of P300 potentials indicate a rather mild cognitive decline that does not accelerate before old age and may be different between both sexes. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1093/gerona/51A.1.B3 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_77967899</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1877135335</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-3094cb655ed76cf4fb12e4991c707927cc6cc7f07752286f909131f897cf4993</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kc1rGzEQxUVoSVIn554KooHe1tbHSlr1tnaSOhCSUAINhSJkWeso2ZUcabfU_33l2uQQ6CCYEe_HG4kHwEeMxhhJOlnZGLyeMFyP8XhKD8AxFqwqGGUP7_KMhCwYQvwIfEjpCW2LkUNwWLGyKiU_Br_qlS2-21b3dgnPN153ziQYGjgLK-9699vCadTOw8vBm94Fn2C-zIdO-_QV1h5etNb0MawfN8mFNqyc0S2s1-sYtHk8Ae8b3SZ7uu8jcH95cT-bF9e3365m9XVhSsL7giJZmgVnzC4FN03ZLDCxpZTYiPwBIozhxogGCcEIqXgjkcQUN5UUGZaSjsCXnW3e-jLY1KvOJWPbVnsbhqSEkFxU_8DPb8CnMESfn6YIqjgpOaEZOvsfhCshMGU0nxGY7CgTQ0rRNmodXafjRmGkttGoXTQqR6Owmm59P-19h0Vnl6_8PousFzvdpd7-eZV1fFZcUMHU_OGnwjf4B75DUjH6F6s8mIA</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877135335</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Age-Related Dynamics of Cognitive Brain Functions in Humans: An Electrophysiological Approach</title><source>Oxford University Press Journals All Titles (1996-Current)</source><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Kügler, Christian F. A. ; Petter, Jürgen ; Platt, Dieter</creator><creatorcontrib>Kügler, Christian F. A. ; Petter, Jürgen ; Platt, Dieter</creatorcontrib><description>Event-related P300 potentials closely reflect cognitive functions such as stimulus discrimination (N250) and processing time (P300 latencies) as well as attention capabilities (P300 amplitudes). To delineate the age-related dynamics of P300 potentials, we investigated 250 healthy subjects between 18 and 98 years of age in a cross-sectional study. A total of 330 visual P300 tests was performed in two different paradigms (PI, passive condition, n = 80; PII, active condition, n = 250). In both P300 paradigms, the N250 and P300 latencies were markedly prolonged (p < .0001) in older age, whereas the N250 and P300 latency differences between PII and PI did not change (p > .05). The P300 amplitudes in paradigm I and II revealed only a slight age-related reduction. In fact, the P300 amplitude ratios between PII and PI remained constant. Third-order polynomial regressions provided the best fit of the aging-P300 interactions in paradigms I and II for both males and females. Interestingly, females showed a greater and possibly earlier P300 latency increase during aging than males. These age-related changes of P300 potentials indicate a rather mild cognitive decline that does not accelerate before old age and may be different between both sexes.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1079-5006</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1758-535X</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1093/gerona/51A.1.B3</identifier><identifier>PMID: 8548496</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: The Gerontological Society of America</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Aged ; Aged, 80 and over ; Aging ; Aging - physiology ; Brain ; Brain - physiology ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognition - physiology ; Cross-Sectional Studies ; Evoked Potentials, Visual ; Female ; Humans ; Male ; Middle Aged ; Regression Analysis</subject><ispartof>The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences, 1996-01, Vol.51A (1), p.B3-B16</ispartof><rights>Copyright Gerontological Society of America, Incorporated Jan 1996</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-3094cb655ed76cf4fb12e4991c707927cc6cc7f07752286f909131f897cf4993</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27846,27901,27902</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8548496$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Kügler, Christian F. A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Petter, Jürgen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Platt, Dieter</creatorcontrib><title>Age-Related Dynamics of Cognitive Brain Functions in Humans: An Electrophysiological Approach</title><title>The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences</title><addtitle>J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci</addtitle><description>Event-related P300 potentials closely reflect cognitive functions such as stimulus discrimination (N250) and processing time (P300 latencies) as well as attention capabilities (P300 amplitudes). To delineate the age-related dynamics of P300 potentials, we investigated 250 healthy subjects between 18 and 98 years of age in a cross-sectional study. A total of 330 visual P300 tests was performed in two different paradigms (PI, passive condition, n = 80; PII, active condition, n = 250). In both P300 paradigms, the N250 and P300 latencies were markedly prolonged (p < .0001) in older age, whereas the N250 and P300 latency differences between PII and PI did not change (p > .05). The P300 amplitudes in paradigm I and II revealed only a slight age-related reduction. In fact, the P300 amplitude ratios between PII and PI remained constant. Third-order polynomial regressions provided the best fit of the aging-P300 interactions in paradigms I and II for both males and females. Interestingly, females showed a greater and possibly earlier P300 latency increase during aging than males. These age-related changes of P300 potentials indicate a rather mild cognitive decline that does not accelerate before old age and may be different between both sexes.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Aged</subject><subject>Aged, 80 and over</subject><subject>Aging</subject><subject>Aging - physiology</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>Cross-Sectional Studies</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials, Visual</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Regression Analysis</subject><issn>1079-5006</issn><issn>1758-535X</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1996</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kc1rGzEQxUVoSVIn554KooHe1tbHSlr1tnaSOhCSUAINhSJkWeso2ZUcabfU_33l2uQQ6CCYEe_HG4kHwEeMxhhJOlnZGLyeMFyP8XhKD8AxFqwqGGUP7_KMhCwYQvwIfEjpCW2LkUNwWLGyKiU_Br_qlS2-21b3dgnPN153ziQYGjgLK-9699vCadTOw8vBm94Fn2C-zIdO-_QV1h5etNb0MawfN8mFNqyc0S2s1-sYtHk8Ae8b3SZ7uu8jcH95cT-bF9e3365m9XVhSsL7giJZmgVnzC4FN03ZLDCxpZTYiPwBIozhxogGCcEIqXgjkcQUN5UUGZaSjsCXnW3e-jLY1KvOJWPbVnsbhqSEkFxU_8DPb8CnMESfn6YIqjgpOaEZOvsfhCshMGU0nxGY7CgTQ0rRNmodXafjRmGkttGoXTQqR6Owmm59P-19h0Vnl6_8PousFzvdpd7-eZV1fFZcUMHU_OGnwjf4B75DUjH6F6s8mIA</recordid><startdate>199601</startdate><enddate>199601</enddate><creator>Kügler, Christian F. A.</creator><creator>Petter, Jürgen</creator><creator>Platt, Dieter</creator><general>The Gerontological Society of America</general><general>Gerontological Society of America</general><general>Oxford University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>199601</creationdate><title>Age-Related Dynamics of Cognitive Brain Functions in Humans: An Electrophysiological Approach</title><author>Kügler, Christian F. A. ; Petter, Jürgen ; Platt, Dieter</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c426t-3094cb655ed76cf4fb12e4991c707927cc6cc7f07752286f909131f897cf4993</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1996</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Aged</topic><topic>Aged, 80 and over</topic><topic>Aging</topic><topic>Aging - physiology</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>Cross-Sectional Studies</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials, Visual</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Regression Analysis</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Kügler, Christian F. A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Petter, Jürgen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Platt, Dieter</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Kügler, Christian F. A.</au><au>Petter, Jürgen</au><au>Platt, Dieter</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Age-Related Dynamics of Cognitive Brain Functions in Humans: An Electrophysiological Approach</atitle><jtitle>The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences</jtitle><addtitle>J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci</addtitle><date>1996-01</date><risdate>1996</risdate><volume>51A</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>B3</spage><epage>B16</epage><pages>B3-B16</pages><issn>1079-5006</issn><eissn>1758-535X</eissn><abstract>Event-related P300 potentials closely reflect cognitive functions such as stimulus discrimination (N250) and processing time (P300 latencies) as well as attention capabilities (P300 amplitudes). To delineate the age-related dynamics of P300 potentials, we investigated 250 healthy subjects between 18 and 98 years of age in a cross-sectional study. A total of 330 visual P300 tests was performed in two different paradigms (PI, passive condition, n = 80; PII, active condition, n = 250). In both P300 paradigms, the N250 and P300 latencies were markedly prolonged (p < .0001) in older age, whereas the N250 and P300 latency differences between PII and PI did not change (p > .05). The P300 amplitudes in paradigm I and II revealed only a slight age-related reduction. In fact, the P300 amplitude ratios between PII and PI remained constant. Third-order polynomial regressions provided the best fit of the aging-P300 interactions in paradigms I and II for both males and females. Interestingly, females showed a greater and possibly earlier P300 latency increase during aging than males. These age-related changes of P300 potentials indicate a rather mild cognitive decline that does not accelerate before old age and may be different between both sexes.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>The Gerontological Society of America</pub><pmid>8548496</pmid><doi>10.1093/gerona/51A.1.B3</doi><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1079-5006 |
ispartof | The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences, 1996-01, Vol.51A (1), p.B3-B16 |
issn | 1079-5006 1758-535X |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_77967899 |
source | Oxford University Press Journals All Titles (1996-Current); MEDLINE; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Aged Aged, 80 and over Aging Aging - physiology Brain Brain - physiology Cognition & reasoning Cognition - physiology Cross-Sectional Studies Evoked Potentials, Visual Female Humans Male Middle Aged Regression Analysis |
title | Age-Related Dynamics of Cognitive Brain Functions in Humans: An Electrophysiological Approach |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-31T01%3A03%3A56IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Age-Related%20Dynamics%20of%20Cognitive%20Brain%20Functions%20in%20Humans:%20An%20Electrophysiological%20Approach&rft.jtitle=The%20journals%20of%20gerontology.%20Series%20A,%20Biological%20sciences%20and%20medical%20sciences&rft.au=K%C3%BCgler,%20Christian%20F.%20A.&rft.date=1996-01&rft.volume=51A&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=B3&rft.epage=B16&rft.pages=B3-B16&rft.issn=1079-5006&rft.eissn=1758-535X&rft_id=info:doi/10.1093/gerona/51A.1.B3&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1877135335%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877135335&rft_id=info:pmid/8548496&rfr_iscdi=true |