Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing

Human performance is seldom perfect, and even when an overt response is correct it may be accompanied by partial-error activity that does not achieve the level of a complete incorrect response. Partial errors can be detected in measures of the lateralized readiness potential, of the electromyogram,...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Acta psychologica 1995-11, Vol.90 (1-3), p.129-144
Hauptverfasser: Coles, Michael G.H., Scheffers, Marten K., Fournier, Lisa
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 144
container_issue 1-3
container_start_page 129
container_title Acta psychologica
container_volume 90
creator Coles, Michael G.H.
Scheffers, Marten K.
Fournier, Lisa
description Human performance is seldom perfect, and even when an overt response is correct it may be accompanied by partial-error activity that does not achieve the level of a complete incorrect response. Partial errors can be detected in measures of the lateralized readiness potential, of the electromyogram, and of response force. Correct responses accompanied by partial errors tend to have slower reaction times than “clean” correct responses (because of response competition), and condition differences in reaction time can, on some occasions, be explained in terms of differences in the incidence of partial errors. In two-choice reaction time tasks, partial errors are more frequent when the imperative stimulus contains information that favors both responses, than when it contains information that favors only one response. The non-random nature of partial errors supports the inference that partial information about the stimulus is used to guide responses. A similar inference is supported by the observation that, in hybrid choice Go/No-go tasks, the kinds of partial errors that follow a No-go stimulus represent activation of the response that would have been correct had the stimulus been a Go stimulus. Finally, we note that the human processing system is capable of monitoring its own behavior and of initiating remedial actions if necessary. The activity of an error-detection system, as revealed by measures of the error-related negativity, is related to the degree to which responses are slowed after errors.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00020-U
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_77712843</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>000169189500020U</els_id><sourcerecordid>32744831</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c498t-b8b60dd9637aafe45a4a3b0d26c0c0bd7500b8191ffaaa24f0268db5b435b6853</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkVtrVDEUhYNU2rH6DxQCBbHg0dyT86JIqRco-OLgmyG3M5MyJxmTc1r67810hj740D6Fxf7W3jt7AfAaow8YYfERIYQ70WP1rufnTRDULZ-BBVaSdoL08ggsHpAT8KLW6yYZ7vExOFaccCXUAvz5vQ4lQB89vMszXGV4W3JafYaXpeRS38OtKVM0GxgO2iQPp3WAyUxzM-YBrufRJBjTkMtoppgT3JbsQq0xrV6C54PZ1PDq8J6C5dfLXxffu6uf335cfLnqHOvV1FllBfK-F1QaMwTGDTPUIk-EQw5ZLzlCVrXVh8EYQ9iAiFDecssot0Jxegre7vu20X_nUCc9xurCZmNSyHPVUkpMFKNPgly2CxGlngQpkYwpiht49h94neeS2m81pggTSknPGsX2lCu51hIGvS1xNOVOY6R3cepdVnqXle65vo9TL5vtzaH5bMfgH0yH_Fr9074e2nFvYii6uhiSCz6W4Cbtc3x8wD8BFq3v</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1301233294</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Coles, Michael G.H. ; Scheffers, Marten K. ; Fournier, Lisa</creator><creatorcontrib>Coles, Michael G.H. ; Scheffers, Marten K. ; Fournier, Lisa</creatorcontrib><description>Human performance is seldom perfect, and even when an overt response is correct it may be accompanied by partial-error activity that does not achieve the level of a complete incorrect response. Partial errors can be detected in measures of the lateralized readiness potential, of the electromyogram, and of response force. Correct responses accompanied by partial errors tend to have slower reaction times than “clean” correct responses (because of response competition), and condition differences in reaction time can, on some occasions, be explained in terms of differences in the incidence of partial errors. In two-choice reaction time tasks, partial errors are more frequent when the imperative stimulus contains information that favors both responses, than when it contains information that favors only one response. The non-random nature of partial errors supports the inference that partial information about the stimulus is used to guide responses. A similar inference is supported by the observation that, in hybrid choice Go/No-go tasks, the kinds of partial errors that follow a No-go stimulus represent activation of the response that would have been correct had the stimulus been a Go stimulus. Finally, we note that the human processing system is capable of monitoring its own behavior and of initiating remedial actions if necessary. The activity of an error-detection system, as revealed by measures of the error-related negativity, is related to the degree to which responses are slowed after errors.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0001-6918</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-6297</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00020-U</identifier><identifier>PMID: 8525868</identifier><identifier>CODEN: APSOA2</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Netherlands: Elsevier B.V</publisher><subject>Analysis ; Arousal ; Attention ; Choice Behavior ; Errors ; Humans ; Information processing ; Lateralized readiness potentials ; Psychomotor Performance ; Reaction Time</subject><ispartof>Acta psychologica, 1995-11, Vol.90 (1-3), p.129-144</ispartof><rights>1995</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c498t-b8b60dd9637aafe45a4a3b0d26c0c0bd7500b8191ffaaa24f0268db5b435b6853</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c498t-b8b60dd9637aafe45a4a3b0d26c0c0bd7500b8191ffaaa24f0268db5b435b6853</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(95)00020-U$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>315,781,785,3551,27873,27928,27929,31004,45999</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8525868$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Coles, Michael G.H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Scheffers, Marten K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fournier, Lisa</creatorcontrib><title>Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing</title><title>Acta psychologica</title><addtitle>Acta Psychol (Amst)</addtitle><description>Human performance is seldom perfect, and even when an overt response is correct it may be accompanied by partial-error activity that does not achieve the level of a complete incorrect response. Partial errors can be detected in measures of the lateralized readiness potential, of the electromyogram, and of response force. Correct responses accompanied by partial errors tend to have slower reaction times than “clean” correct responses (because of response competition), and condition differences in reaction time can, on some occasions, be explained in terms of differences in the incidence of partial errors. In two-choice reaction time tasks, partial errors are more frequent when the imperative stimulus contains information that favors both responses, than when it contains information that favors only one response. The non-random nature of partial errors supports the inference that partial information about the stimulus is used to guide responses. A similar inference is supported by the observation that, in hybrid choice Go/No-go tasks, the kinds of partial errors that follow a No-go stimulus represent activation of the response that would have been correct had the stimulus been a Go stimulus. Finally, we note that the human processing system is capable of monitoring its own behavior and of initiating remedial actions if necessary. The activity of an error-detection system, as revealed by measures of the error-related negativity, is related to the degree to which responses are slowed after errors.</description><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Arousal</subject><subject>Attention</subject><subject>Choice Behavior</subject><subject>Errors</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Information processing</subject><subject>Lateralized readiness potentials</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance</subject><subject>Reaction Time</subject><issn>0001-6918</issn><issn>1873-6297</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1995</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkVtrVDEUhYNU2rH6DxQCBbHg0dyT86JIqRco-OLgmyG3M5MyJxmTc1r67810hj740D6Fxf7W3jt7AfAaow8YYfERIYQ70WP1rufnTRDULZ-BBVaSdoL08ggsHpAT8KLW6yYZ7vExOFaccCXUAvz5vQ4lQB89vMszXGV4W3JafYaXpeRS38OtKVM0GxgO2iQPp3WAyUxzM-YBrufRJBjTkMtoppgT3JbsQq0xrV6C54PZ1PDq8J6C5dfLXxffu6uf335cfLnqHOvV1FllBfK-F1QaMwTGDTPUIk-EQw5ZLzlCVrXVh8EYQ9iAiFDecssot0Jxegre7vu20X_nUCc9xurCZmNSyHPVUkpMFKNPgly2CxGlngQpkYwpiht49h94neeS2m81pggTSknPGsX2lCu51hIGvS1xNOVOY6R3cepdVnqXle65vo9TL5vtzaH5bMfgH0yH_Fr9074e2nFvYii6uhiSCz6W4Cbtc3x8wD8BFq3v</recordid><startdate>19951101</startdate><enddate>19951101</enddate><creator>Coles, Michael G.H.</creator><creator>Scheffers, Marten K.</creator><creator>Fournier, Lisa</creator><general>Elsevier B.V</general><general>Martinus Nijhoff</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>ICWRT</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7TA</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>JG9</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19951101</creationdate><title>Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing</title><author>Coles, Michael G.H. ; Scheffers, Marten K. ; Fournier, Lisa</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c498t-b8b60dd9637aafe45a4a3b0d26c0c0bd7500b8191ffaaa24f0268db5b435b6853</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1995</creationdate><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Arousal</topic><topic>Attention</topic><topic>Choice Behavior</topic><topic>Errors</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Information processing</topic><topic>Lateralized readiness potentials</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance</topic><topic>Reaction Time</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Coles, Michael G.H.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Scheffers, Marten K.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Fournier, Lisa</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 28</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Materials Business File</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Materials Research Database</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Acta psychologica</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Coles, Michael G.H.</au><au>Scheffers, Marten K.</au><au>Fournier, Lisa</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing</atitle><jtitle>Acta psychologica</jtitle><addtitle>Acta Psychol (Amst)</addtitle><date>1995-11-01</date><risdate>1995</risdate><volume>90</volume><issue>1-3</issue><spage>129</spage><epage>144</epage><pages>129-144</pages><issn>0001-6918</issn><eissn>1873-6297</eissn><coden>APSOA2</coden><abstract>Human performance is seldom perfect, and even when an overt response is correct it may be accompanied by partial-error activity that does not achieve the level of a complete incorrect response. Partial errors can be detected in measures of the lateralized readiness potential, of the electromyogram, and of response force. Correct responses accompanied by partial errors tend to have slower reaction times than “clean” correct responses (because of response competition), and condition differences in reaction time can, on some occasions, be explained in terms of differences in the incidence of partial errors. In two-choice reaction time tasks, partial errors are more frequent when the imperative stimulus contains information that favors both responses, than when it contains information that favors only one response. The non-random nature of partial errors supports the inference that partial information about the stimulus is used to guide responses. A similar inference is supported by the observation that, in hybrid choice Go/No-go tasks, the kinds of partial errors that follow a No-go stimulus represent activation of the response that would have been correct had the stimulus been a Go stimulus. Finally, we note that the human processing system is capable of monitoring its own behavior and of initiating remedial actions if necessary. The activity of an error-detection system, as revealed by measures of the error-related negativity, is related to the degree to which responses are slowed after errors.</abstract><cop>Netherlands</cop><pub>Elsevier B.V</pub><pmid>8525868</pmid><doi>10.1016/0001-6918(95)00020-U</doi><tpages>16</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0001-6918
ispartof Acta psychologica, 1995-11, Vol.90 (1-3), p.129-144
issn 0001-6918
1873-6297
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_77712843
source MEDLINE; Periodicals Index Online; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)
subjects Analysis
Arousal
Attention
Choice Behavior
Errors
Humans
Information processing
Lateralized readiness potentials
Psychomotor Performance
Reaction Time
title Where did you go wrong? Errors, partial errors, and the nature of human information processing
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-16T14%3A09%3A01IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Where%20did%20you%20go%20wrong?%20Errors,%20partial%20errors,%20and%20the%20nature%20of%20human%20information%20processing&rft.jtitle=Acta%20psychologica&rft.au=Coles,%20Michael%20G.H.&rft.date=1995-11-01&rft.volume=90&rft.issue=1-3&rft.spage=129&rft.epage=144&rft.pages=129-144&rft.issn=0001-6918&rft.eissn=1873-6297&rft.coden=APSOA2&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/0001-6918(95)00020-U&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E32744831%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1301233294&rft_id=info:pmid/8525868&rft_els_id=000169189500020U&rfr_iscdi=true