Sleep loss does not aggravate the deteriorating effect of hypoglycemia on neurocognitive function in healthy men

Summary Introduction Sleep deprivation (SD) impairs neurocognitive functions. Assuming that this effect is mediated by reduced cerebral glucose supply due to prolonged wakefulness inducing a progressive depletion of cerebral glycogen stores, we hypothesized that short-term sleep loss amplifies the d...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010-05, Vol.35 (4), p.624-628
Hauptverfasser: Jauch-Chara, Kamila, Hallschmid, Manfred, Schmid, Sebastian M, Bandorf, Nadine, Born, Jan, Schultes, Bernd
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 628
container_issue 4
container_start_page 624
container_title Psychoneuroendocrinology
container_volume 35
creator Jauch-Chara, Kamila
Hallschmid, Manfred
Schmid, Sebastian M
Bandorf, Nadine
Born, Jan
Schultes, Bernd
description Summary Introduction Sleep deprivation (SD) impairs neurocognitive functions. Assuming that this effect is mediated by reduced cerebral glucose supply due to prolonged wakefulness inducing a progressive depletion of cerebral glycogen stores, we hypothesized that short-term sleep loss amplifies the deteriorating effects of acute hypoglycemia on neurocognitive functions. Methods Seven healthy men were tested in a randomized and balanced order on 3 different conditions spaced 2 weeks apart. After a night of total SD (total SD), 4.5 h of sleep (partial SD) and a night with 7 h of regular sleep (regular sleep), subjects were exposed to a stepwise hypoglycemic clamp experiment. Reaction time (RT) and auditory evoked brain potentials (AEP) were assessed during a euglycemic baseline period and at the end of the clamp (blood glucose at 2.5 mmol/l). Results During the euglycemic baseline, amplitude of the P3 component of the AEP was lower after total SD than after partial SD (9.2 ± 3.2 μV vs. 16.6 ± 2.9 μV; t (6) = 3.2, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (20.2 ± 2.1 μV; t (6) = 18.8, P < 0.01). Reaction time was longer after total SD in comparison to partial SD (367 ± 45 ms vs. 304 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.7, P = 0.04) and to regular sleep (322 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.41, P = 0.06) while there was no difference between partial SD and regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 0.60, P = 0.57). Hypoglycemia decreased P3 amplitude by 11.2 ± 4.1 μV in the partial SD condition ( t (6) = 2.72, P = 0.04) and by 9.3 ± 0.7 μV in the regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 12.51, P < 0.01), but did not further reduce P3 amplitude after total SD (1.8 ± 3.9 μV; t (6) = 0.46, P = 0.66). Thus, at the end of hypoglycemia P3 amplitudes were similar across the 3 conditions ( F (2,10) = 0.89, P = 0.42). RT generally showed a similar pattern with a significant prolongation due to hypoglycemia after partial SD (+42 ± 12 ms; t (6) = 3.39, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (+37 ± 10 ms; t (6) = 3.53, P = 0.01), but not after total SD (+15 ± 16; t (6) = 0.97, P = 0.37), resulting in similar values at the end of hypoglycemia ( F (1,6) = 1.01, P = 0.36). Conclusions One night of total SD deteriorates neurocognitive function as reflected by indicators of attentive stimulus processing, but does not synergistically aggravate the impairing influence of acute hypoglycemia. The findings are not consistent with the view that neurocognitive deteriorations after SD result from challenged cerebral glucose metabolism.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.018
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_744616054</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0306453009002959</els_id><sourcerecordid>744616054</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c484t-96a58f10f4dfdc0dcca1d766685f49441d78b4b8b85d06be53f51cad1240bb4b3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFksGKFDEQhhtR3NnVV1hyEU8zVjrpTPoiyqKusOBh9RzS6cpMxp6kTdID_fammVHBy0JBSPLVX0X9VVW3FDYUqHh32Ixp9jih39QA7WYJKp9VKyq3bM2YgOfVChiINW8YXFXXKR0AQEhRv6yuaCuZkG27qsbHAXEkQ0iJ9AET8SETvdtFfdIZSd4j6TFjdCHq7PyOoLVoMgmW7Ocx7IbZ4NFpEjwp3cRgws677E5I7ORNduXdebJHPeT9TI7oX1UvrB4Svr6cN9WPz5--392vH759-Xr38WFtuOR53QrdSEvB8t72BnpjNO23QgjZWN5yXi6y453sZNOD6LBhtqFG97Tm0JUPdlO9PeuOMfyaMGV1dMngMGiPYUpqy7mgAhr-NMkY41JKWkhxJk0s84po1RjdUcdZUVCLLeqg_tiiFlvUElSWxNtLiak7Yv8v7eJDAd5cAJ2MHmzU3rj0l6vrRjZQL0IfzhyW0Z0cRpWMQ2-wd7HYovrgnu7l_X8SZnDelao_ccZ0CFP0xRhFVaoVqMdliZYdghagbpuW_QbO_8ZL</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>733348881</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Sleep loss does not aggravate the deteriorating effect of hypoglycemia on neurocognitive function in healthy men</title><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete - AutoHoldings</source><source>MEDLINE</source><creator>Jauch-Chara, Kamila ; Hallschmid, Manfred ; Schmid, Sebastian M ; Bandorf, Nadine ; Born, Jan ; Schultes, Bernd</creator><creatorcontrib>Jauch-Chara, Kamila ; Hallschmid, Manfred ; Schmid, Sebastian M ; Bandorf, Nadine ; Born, Jan ; Schultes, Bernd</creatorcontrib><description>Summary Introduction Sleep deprivation (SD) impairs neurocognitive functions. Assuming that this effect is mediated by reduced cerebral glucose supply due to prolonged wakefulness inducing a progressive depletion of cerebral glycogen stores, we hypothesized that short-term sleep loss amplifies the deteriorating effects of acute hypoglycemia on neurocognitive functions. Methods Seven healthy men were tested in a randomized and balanced order on 3 different conditions spaced 2 weeks apart. After a night of total SD (total SD), 4.5 h of sleep (partial SD) and a night with 7 h of regular sleep (regular sleep), subjects were exposed to a stepwise hypoglycemic clamp experiment. Reaction time (RT) and auditory evoked brain potentials (AEP) were assessed during a euglycemic baseline period and at the end of the clamp (blood glucose at 2.5 mmol/l). Results During the euglycemic baseline, amplitude of the P3 component of the AEP was lower after total SD than after partial SD (9.2 ± 3.2 μV vs. 16.6 ± 2.9 μV; t (6) = 3.2, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (20.2 ± 2.1 μV; t (6) = 18.8, P &lt; 0.01). Reaction time was longer after total SD in comparison to partial SD (367 ± 45 ms vs. 304 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.7, P = 0.04) and to regular sleep (322 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.41, P = 0.06) while there was no difference between partial SD and regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 0.60, P = 0.57). Hypoglycemia decreased P3 amplitude by 11.2 ± 4.1 μV in the partial SD condition ( t (6) = 2.72, P = 0.04) and by 9.3 ± 0.7 μV in the regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 12.51, P &lt; 0.01), but did not further reduce P3 amplitude after total SD (1.8 ± 3.9 μV; t (6) = 0.46, P = 0.66). Thus, at the end of hypoglycemia P3 amplitudes were similar across the 3 conditions ( F (2,10) = 0.89, P = 0.42). RT generally showed a similar pattern with a significant prolongation due to hypoglycemia after partial SD (+42 ± 12 ms; t (6) = 3.39, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (+37 ± 10 ms; t (6) = 3.53, P = 0.01), but not after total SD (+15 ± 16; t (6) = 0.97, P = 0.37), resulting in similar values at the end of hypoglycemia ( F (1,6) = 1.01, P = 0.36). Conclusions One night of total SD deteriorates neurocognitive function as reflected by indicators of attentive stimulus processing, but does not synergistically aggravate the impairing influence of acute hypoglycemia. The findings are not consistent with the view that neurocognitive deteriorations after SD result from challenged cerebral glucose metabolism.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0306-4530</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-3360</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.018</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19836899</identifier><identifier>CODEN: PSYCDE</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Kidlington: Elsevier Ltd</publisher><subject>Adult ; Auditory evoked brain potentials ; Behavioral psychophysiology ; Biological and medical sciences ; Cognition - physiology ; Cognition Disorders - etiology ; Cognition Disorders - physiopathology ; Disorders of higher nervous function. Focal brain diseases. Central vestibular syndrome and deafness. Brain stem syndromes ; Electroencephalography ; Endocrinology &amp; Metabolism ; Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Health ; Hormones and behavior ; Humans ; Hypoglycemia - chemically induced ; Hypoglycemia - complications ; Hypoglycemia - physiopathology ; Hypoglycemic Agents - administration &amp; dosage ; Insulin Infusion Systems ; Male ; Medical sciences ; Mood ; Nervous system (semeiology, syndromes) ; Neuroglycopenia ; Neurology ; Neuropsychological Tests ; P3 amplitude ; Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Risk Factors ; Sleep deprivation ; Sleep Deprivation - complications ; Sleep Deprivation - physiopathology ; Task Performance and Analysis ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2010-05, Vol.35 (4), p.624-628</ispartof><rights>Elsevier Ltd</rights><rights>2009 Elsevier Ltd</rights><rights>2015 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c484t-96a58f10f4dfdc0dcca1d766685f49441d78b4b8b85d06be53f51cad1240bb4b3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c484t-96a58f10f4dfdc0dcca1d766685f49441d78b4b8b85d06be53f51cad1240bb4b3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.018$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3548,27922,27923,45993</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=22585028$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19836899$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Jauch-Chara, Kamila</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hallschmid, Manfred</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schmid, Sebastian M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bandorf, Nadine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Born, Jan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schultes, Bernd</creatorcontrib><title>Sleep loss does not aggravate the deteriorating effect of hypoglycemia on neurocognitive function in healthy men</title><title>Psychoneuroendocrinology</title><addtitle>Psychoneuroendocrinology</addtitle><description>Summary Introduction Sleep deprivation (SD) impairs neurocognitive functions. Assuming that this effect is mediated by reduced cerebral glucose supply due to prolonged wakefulness inducing a progressive depletion of cerebral glycogen stores, we hypothesized that short-term sleep loss amplifies the deteriorating effects of acute hypoglycemia on neurocognitive functions. Methods Seven healthy men were tested in a randomized and balanced order on 3 different conditions spaced 2 weeks apart. After a night of total SD (total SD), 4.5 h of sleep (partial SD) and a night with 7 h of regular sleep (regular sleep), subjects were exposed to a stepwise hypoglycemic clamp experiment. Reaction time (RT) and auditory evoked brain potentials (AEP) were assessed during a euglycemic baseline period and at the end of the clamp (blood glucose at 2.5 mmol/l). Results During the euglycemic baseline, amplitude of the P3 component of the AEP was lower after total SD than after partial SD (9.2 ± 3.2 μV vs. 16.6 ± 2.9 μV; t (6) = 3.2, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (20.2 ± 2.1 μV; t (6) = 18.8, P &lt; 0.01). Reaction time was longer after total SD in comparison to partial SD (367 ± 45 ms vs. 304 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.7, P = 0.04) and to regular sleep (322 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.41, P = 0.06) while there was no difference between partial SD and regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 0.60, P = 0.57). Hypoglycemia decreased P3 amplitude by 11.2 ± 4.1 μV in the partial SD condition ( t (6) = 2.72, P = 0.04) and by 9.3 ± 0.7 μV in the regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 12.51, P &lt; 0.01), but did not further reduce P3 amplitude after total SD (1.8 ± 3.9 μV; t (6) = 0.46, P = 0.66). Thus, at the end of hypoglycemia P3 amplitudes were similar across the 3 conditions ( F (2,10) = 0.89, P = 0.42). RT generally showed a similar pattern with a significant prolongation due to hypoglycemia after partial SD (+42 ± 12 ms; t (6) = 3.39, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (+37 ± 10 ms; t (6) = 3.53, P = 0.01), but not after total SD (+15 ± 16; t (6) = 0.97, P = 0.37), resulting in similar values at the end of hypoglycemia ( F (1,6) = 1.01, P = 0.36). Conclusions One night of total SD deteriorates neurocognitive function as reflected by indicators of attentive stimulus processing, but does not synergistically aggravate the impairing influence of acute hypoglycemia. The findings are not consistent with the view that neurocognitive deteriorations after SD result from challenged cerebral glucose metabolism.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Auditory evoked brain potentials</subject><subject>Behavioral psychophysiology</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>Cognition Disorders - etiology</subject><subject>Cognition Disorders - physiopathology</subject><subject>Disorders of higher nervous function. Focal brain diseases. Central vestibular syndrome and deafness. Brain stem syndromes</subject><subject>Electroencephalography</subject><subject>Endocrinology &amp; Metabolism</subject><subject>Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Health</subject><subject>Hormones and behavior</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Hypoglycemia - chemically induced</subject><subject>Hypoglycemia - complications</subject><subject>Hypoglycemia - physiopathology</subject><subject>Hypoglycemic Agents - administration &amp; dosage</subject><subject>Insulin Infusion Systems</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical sciences</subject><subject>Mood</subject><subject>Nervous system (semeiology, syndromes)</subject><subject>Neuroglycopenia</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neuropsychological Tests</subject><subject>P3 amplitude</subject><subject>Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Risk Factors</subject><subject>Sleep deprivation</subject><subject>Sleep Deprivation - complications</subject><subject>Sleep Deprivation - physiopathology</subject><subject>Task Performance and Analysis</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>0306-4530</issn><issn>1873-3360</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2010</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFksGKFDEQhhtR3NnVV1hyEU8zVjrpTPoiyqKusOBh9RzS6cpMxp6kTdID_fammVHBy0JBSPLVX0X9VVW3FDYUqHh32Ixp9jih39QA7WYJKp9VKyq3bM2YgOfVChiINW8YXFXXKR0AQEhRv6yuaCuZkG27qsbHAXEkQ0iJ9AET8SETvdtFfdIZSd4j6TFjdCHq7PyOoLVoMgmW7Ocx7IbZ4NFpEjwp3cRgws677E5I7ORNduXdebJHPeT9TI7oX1UvrB4Svr6cN9WPz5--392vH759-Xr38WFtuOR53QrdSEvB8t72BnpjNO23QgjZWN5yXi6y453sZNOD6LBhtqFG97Tm0JUPdlO9PeuOMfyaMGV1dMngMGiPYUpqy7mgAhr-NMkY41JKWkhxJk0s84po1RjdUcdZUVCLLeqg_tiiFlvUElSWxNtLiak7Yv8v7eJDAd5cAJ2MHmzU3rj0l6vrRjZQL0IfzhyW0Z0cRpWMQ2-wd7HYovrgnu7l_X8SZnDelao_ccZ0CFP0xRhFVaoVqMdliZYdghagbpuW_QbO_8ZL</recordid><startdate>20100501</startdate><enddate>20100501</enddate><creator>Jauch-Chara, Kamila</creator><creator>Hallschmid, Manfred</creator><creator>Schmid, Sebastian M</creator><creator>Bandorf, Nadine</creator><creator>Born, Jan</creator><creator>Schultes, Bernd</creator><general>Elsevier Ltd</general><general>Elsevier</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>7TK</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20100501</creationdate><title>Sleep loss does not aggravate the deteriorating effect of hypoglycemia on neurocognitive function in healthy men</title><author>Jauch-Chara, Kamila ; Hallschmid, Manfred ; Schmid, Sebastian M ; Bandorf, Nadine ; Born, Jan ; Schultes, Bernd</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c484t-96a58f10f4dfdc0dcca1d766685f49441d78b4b8b85d06be53f51cad1240bb4b3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2010</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Auditory evoked brain potentials</topic><topic>Behavioral psychophysiology</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>Cognition Disorders - etiology</topic><topic>Cognition Disorders - physiopathology</topic><topic>Disorders of higher nervous function. Focal brain diseases. Central vestibular syndrome and deafness. Brain stem syndromes</topic><topic>Electroencephalography</topic><topic>Endocrinology &amp; Metabolism</topic><topic>Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Health</topic><topic>Hormones and behavior</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Hypoglycemia - chemically induced</topic><topic>Hypoglycemia - complications</topic><topic>Hypoglycemia - physiopathology</topic><topic>Hypoglycemic Agents - administration &amp; dosage</topic><topic>Insulin Infusion Systems</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical sciences</topic><topic>Mood</topic><topic>Nervous system (semeiology, syndromes)</topic><topic>Neuroglycopenia</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neuropsychological Tests</topic><topic>P3 amplitude</topic><topic>Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Risk Factors</topic><topic>Sleep deprivation</topic><topic>Sleep Deprivation - complications</topic><topic>Sleep Deprivation - physiopathology</topic><topic>Task Performance and Analysis</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Jauch-Chara, Kamila</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Hallschmid, Manfred</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schmid, Sebastian M</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Bandorf, Nadine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Born, Jan</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schultes, Bernd</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><jtitle>Psychoneuroendocrinology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Jauch-Chara, Kamila</au><au>Hallschmid, Manfred</au><au>Schmid, Sebastian M</au><au>Bandorf, Nadine</au><au>Born, Jan</au><au>Schultes, Bernd</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Sleep loss does not aggravate the deteriorating effect of hypoglycemia on neurocognitive function in healthy men</atitle><jtitle>Psychoneuroendocrinology</jtitle><addtitle>Psychoneuroendocrinology</addtitle><date>2010-05-01</date><risdate>2010</risdate><volume>35</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>624</spage><epage>628</epage><pages>624-628</pages><issn>0306-4530</issn><eissn>1873-3360</eissn><coden>PSYCDE</coden><abstract>Summary Introduction Sleep deprivation (SD) impairs neurocognitive functions. Assuming that this effect is mediated by reduced cerebral glucose supply due to prolonged wakefulness inducing a progressive depletion of cerebral glycogen stores, we hypothesized that short-term sleep loss amplifies the deteriorating effects of acute hypoglycemia on neurocognitive functions. Methods Seven healthy men were tested in a randomized and balanced order on 3 different conditions spaced 2 weeks apart. After a night of total SD (total SD), 4.5 h of sleep (partial SD) and a night with 7 h of regular sleep (regular sleep), subjects were exposed to a stepwise hypoglycemic clamp experiment. Reaction time (RT) and auditory evoked brain potentials (AEP) were assessed during a euglycemic baseline period and at the end of the clamp (blood glucose at 2.5 mmol/l). Results During the euglycemic baseline, amplitude of the P3 component of the AEP was lower after total SD than after partial SD (9.2 ± 3.2 μV vs. 16.6 ± 2.9 μV; t (6) = 3.2, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (20.2 ± 2.1 μV; t (6) = 18.8, P &lt; 0.01). Reaction time was longer after total SD in comparison to partial SD (367 ± 45 ms vs. 304 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.7, P = 0.04) and to regular sleep (322 ± 36 ms; t (6) = 2.41, P = 0.06) while there was no difference between partial SD and regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 0.60, P = 0.57). Hypoglycemia decreased P3 amplitude by 11.2 ± 4.1 μV in the partial SD condition ( t (6) = 2.72, P = 0.04) and by 9.3 ± 0.7 μV in the regular sleep condition ( t (6) = 12.51, P &lt; 0.01), but did not further reduce P3 amplitude after total SD (1.8 ± 3.9 μV; t (6) = 0.46, P = 0.66). Thus, at the end of hypoglycemia P3 amplitudes were similar across the 3 conditions ( F (2,10) = 0.89, P = 0.42). RT generally showed a similar pattern with a significant prolongation due to hypoglycemia after partial SD (+42 ± 12 ms; t (6) = 3.39, P = 0.02) and regular sleep (+37 ± 10 ms; t (6) = 3.53, P = 0.01), but not after total SD (+15 ± 16; t (6) = 0.97, P = 0.37), resulting in similar values at the end of hypoglycemia ( F (1,6) = 1.01, P = 0.36). Conclusions One night of total SD deteriorates neurocognitive function as reflected by indicators of attentive stimulus processing, but does not synergistically aggravate the impairing influence of acute hypoglycemia. The findings are not consistent with the view that neurocognitive deteriorations after SD result from challenged cerebral glucose metabolism.</abstract><cop>Kidlington</cop><pub>Elsevier Ltd</pub><pmid>19836899</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.018</doi><tpages>5</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0306-4530
ispartof Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2010-05, Vol.35 (4), p.624-628
issn 0306-4530
1873-3360
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_744616054
source Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals Complete - AutoHoldings; MEDLINE
subjects Adult
Auditory evoked brain potentials
Behavioral psychophysiology
Biological and medical sciences
Cognition - physiology
Cognition Disorders - etiology
Cognition Disorders - physiopathology
Disorders of higher nervous function. Focal brain diseases. Central vestibular syndrome and deafness. Brain stem syndromes
Electroencephalography
Endocrinology & Metabolism
Evoked Potentials, Auditory - physiology
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Health
Hormones and behavior
Humans
Hypoglycemia - chemically induced
Hypoglycemia - complications
Hypoglycemia - physiopathology
Hypoglycemic Agents - administration & dosage
Insulin Infusion Systems
Male
Medical sciences
Mood
Nervous system (semeiology, syndromes)
Neuroglycopenia
Neurology
Neuropsychological Tests
P3 amplitude
Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Risk Factors
Sleep deprivation
Sleep Deprivation - complications
Sleep Deprivation - physiopathology
Task Performance and Analysis
Young Adult
title Sleep loss does not aggravate the deteriorating effect of hypoglycemia on neurocognitive function in healthy men
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-09T14%3A24%3A12IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Sleep%20loss%20does%20not%20aggravate%20the%20deteriorating%20effect%20of%20hypoglycemia%20on%20neurocognitive%20function%20in%20healthy%20men&rft.jtitle=Psychoneuroendocrinology&rft.au=Jauch-Chara,%20Kamila&rft.date=2010-05-01&rft.volume=35&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=624&rft.epage=628&rft.pages=624-628&rft.issn=0306-4530&rft.eissn=1873-3360&rft.coden=PSYCDE&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.018&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E744616054%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=733348881&rft_id=info:pmid/19836899&rft_els_id=S0306453009002959&rfr_iscdi=true