Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention

Acetylcholine plays a major role in mediating attention processes. We investigated the muscarinic antagonist effect of scopolamine on functional neuro-anatomy of attention and cognition. We assessed 12 healthy volunteers while performing the Attention Network Task on 0.4 mg scopolamine and placebo i...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology 2009-11, Vol.12 (10), p.1307-1317
Hauptverfasser: Thienel, Renate, Kellermann, Thilo, Schall, Ulrich, Voss, Bianca, Reske, Martina, Halfter, Sarah, Sheldrick, Abigail J., Radenbach, Katrin, Habel, Ute, Jon Shah, Nadim, Kircher, Tilo
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 1317
container_issue 10
container_start_page 1307
container_title The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology
container_volume 12
creator Thienel, Renate
Kellermann, Thilo
Schall, Ulrich
Voss, Bianca
Reske, Martina
Halfter, Sarah
Sheldrick, Abigail J.
Radenbach, Katrin
Habel, Ute
Jon Shah, Nadim
Kircher, Tilo
description Acetylcholine plays a major role in mediating attention processes. We investigated the muscarinic antagonist effect of scopolamine on functional neuro-anatomy of attention and cognition. We assessed 12 healthy volunteers while performing the Attention Network Task on 0.4 mg scopolamine and placebo in a single-blind randomized trial in a 1.5 T magnetic resonance scanner. Neurocognitive measures included verbal learning, verbal memory, verbal fluency, trail making, digit span, a continuous performance task and a planning task (Tower of London). When compared to placebo, scopolamine increased reaction times for conflicting stimulus processing, together with decreasing brain activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (a brain region involved in conflict processing) suggestive of a muscarinic antagonist effect on executive control of attention. Contrary to the notion of a predominantly right-hemispheric lateralization of cognitive processes associated with orienting attention, scopolamine reduced brain activity in left superior and left middle frontal brain areas. Our neuropsychological test data revealed a selective effect of scopolamine on verbal learning and memory while other cognitive domains, such as planning and working memory, were unaffected. These findings are consistent with muscarinic modulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission in frontal attention networks when processing conflicting information.
doi_str_mv 10.1017/S146114570999068X
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_733471406</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S146114570999068X</cupid><sourcerecordid>21146475</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c559t-f6d17d4a4c3b4ff4a06ccd02db3185268c6f87f70567be48a9fbf3fb05c09a073</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kctKBTEMhoso3h_AjQwudDWaTG_TlYh4A8WFCu6GTqeVkXNabTuib-8czwFB0VVC8uUP-UPIDsIhAsqjO2QCkXEJSikQ9eMSWR9LquSIuPyVYznrr5GNlJ4BKsapWCVrqKSiDKp1cnwzJKNj73tTaJ_1U_B9yoV1zpqciuAL-27NkPs3W5jgcwyTIrhC52x97oPfIitOT5LdXsRN8nB-dn96WV7fXlydnlyXhnOVSyc6lB3TzNCWOcc0CGM6qLqWYs0rURvhaukkcCFby2qtXOuoa4EbUBok3SQHc92XGF4Hm3Iz7ZOxk4n2NgypkZQyiQzESO7_S1ajY4JJPoJ7P8DnMEQ_XtFUVUUFANYjhHPIxJBStK55if1Ux48GoZk9ofn1hHFmdyE8tFPbfU8sXB8BuhDV0zb23ZP9Xv237CcaO5Df</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>222360018</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Access via Oxford University Press (Open Access Collection)</source><source>EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals</source><creator>Thienel, Renate ; Kellermann, Thilo ; Schall, Ulrich ; Voss, Bianca ; Reske, Martina ; Halfter, Sarah ; Sheldrick, Abigail J. ; Radenbach, Katrin ; Habel, Ute ; Jon Shah, Nadim ; Kircher, Tilo</creator><creatorcontrib>Thienel, Renate ; Kellermann, Thilo ; Schall, Ulrich ; Voss, Bianca ; Reske, Martina ; Halfter, Sarah ; Sheldrick, Abigail J. ; Radenbach, Katrin ; Habel, Ute ; Jon Shah, Nadim ; Kircher, Tilo</creatorcontrib><description>Acetylcholine plays a major role in mediating attention processes. We investigated the muscarinic antagonist effect of scopolamine on functional neuro-anatomy of attention and cognition. We assessed 12 healthy volunteers while performing the Attention Network Task on 0.4 mg scopolamine and placebo in a single-blind randomized trial in a 1.5 T magnetic resonance scanner. Neurocognitive measures included verbal learning, verbal memory, verbal fluency, trail making, digit span, a continuous performance task and a planning task (Tower of London). When compared to placebo, scopolamine increased reaction times for conflicting stimulus processing, together with decreasing brain activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (a brain region involved in conflict processing) suggestive of a muscarinic antagonist effect on executive control of attention. Contrary to the notion of a predominantly right-hemispheric lateralization of cognitive processes associated with orienting attention, scopolamine reduced brain activity in left superior and left middle frontal brain areas. Our neuropsychological test data revealed a selective effect of scopolamine on verbal learning and memory while other cognitive domains, such as planning and working memory, were unaffected. These findings are consistent with muscarinic modulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission in frontal attention networks when processing conflicting information.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1461-1457</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-5111</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S146114570999068X</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19793402</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>Adult ; Attention - drug effects ; Attention - physiology ; Cross-Over Studies ; Executive Function - drug effects ; Executive Function - physiology ; Humans ; Male ; Muscarinic Antagonists - pharmacology ; Psychomotor Performance - drug effects ; Psychomotor Performance - physiology ; Reaction Time - drug effects ; Reaction Time - physiology ; Scopolamine Hydrobromide - pharmacology ; Single-Blind Method ; Young Adult</subject><ispartof>The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology, 2009-11, Vol.12 (10), p.1307-1317</ispartof><rights>Copyright © CINP 2009</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c559t-f6d17d4a4c3b4ff4a06ccd02db3185268c6f87f70567be48a9fbf3fb05c09a073</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c559t-f6d17d4a4c3b4ff4a06ccd02db3185268c6f87f70567be48a9fbf3fb05c09a073</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19793402$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Thienel, Renate</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kellermann, Thilo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schall, Ulrich</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Voss, Bianca</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Reske, Martina</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Halfter, Sarah</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sheldrick, Abigail J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Radenbach, Katrin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Habel, Ute</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jon Shah, Nadim</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kircher, Tilo</creatorcontrib><title>Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention</title><title>The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology</title><addtitle>Int J Neuropsychopharmacol</addtitle><description>Acetylcholine plays a major role in mediating attention processes. We investigated the muscarinic antagonist effect of scopolamine on functional neuro-anatomy of attention and cognition. We assessed 12 healthy volunteers while performing the Attention Network Task on 0.4 mg scopolamine and placebo in a single-blind randomized trial in a 1.5 T magnetic resonance scanner. Neurocognitive measures included verbal learning, verbal memory, verbal fluency, trail making, digit span, a continuous performance task and a planning task (Tower of London). When compared to placebo, scopolamine increased reaction times for conflicting stimulus processing, together with decreasing brain activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (a brain region involved in conflict processing) suggestive of a muscarinic antagonist effect on executive control of attention. Contrary to the notion of a predominantly right-hemispheric lateralization of cognitive processes associated with orienting attention, scopolamine reduced brain activity in left superior and left middle frontal brain areas. Our neuropsychological test data revealed a selective effect of scopolamine on verbal learning and memory while other cognitive domains, such as planning and working memory, were unaffected. These findings are consistent with muscarinic modulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission in frontal attention networks when processing conflicting information.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Attention - drug effects</subject><subject>Attention - physiology</subject><subject>Cross-Over Studies</subject><subject>Executive Function - drug effects</subject><subject>Executive Function - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Muscarinic Antagonists - pharmacology</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - drug effects</subject><subject>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</subject><subject>Reaction Time - drug effects</subject><subject>Reaction Time - physiology</subject><subject>Scopolamine Hydrobromide - pharmacology</subject><subject>Single-Blind Method</subject><subject>Young Adult</subject><issn>1461-1457</issn><issn>1469-5111</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2009</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kctKBTEMhoso3h_AjQwudDWaTG_TlYh4A8WFCu6GTqeVkXNabTuib-8czwFB0VVC8uUP-UPIDsIhAsqjO2QCkXEJSikQ9eMSWR9LquSIuPyVYznrr5GNlJ4BKsapWCVrqKSiDKp1cnwzJKNj73tTaJ_1U_B9yoV1zpqciuAL-27NkPs3W5jgcwyTIrhC52x97oPfIitOT5LdXsRN8nB-dn96WV7fXlydnlyXhnOVSyc6lB3TzNCWOcc0CGM6qLqWYs0rURvhaukkcCFby2qtXOuoa4EbUBok3SQHc92XGF4Hm3Iz7ZOxk4n2NgypkZQyiQzESO7_S1ajY4JJPoJ7P8DnMEQ_XtFUVUUFANYjhHPIxJBStK55if1Ux48GoZk9ofn1hHFmdyE8tFPbfU8sXB8BuhDV0zb23ZP9Xv237CcaO5Df</recordid><startdate>20091101</startdate><enddate>20091101</enddate><creator>Thienel, Renate</creator><creator>Kellermann, Thilo</creator><creator>Schall, Ulrich</creator><creator>Voss, Bianca</creator><creator>Reske, Martina</creator><creator>Halfter, Sarah</creator><creator>Sheldrick, Abigail J.</creator><creator>Radenbach, Katrin</creator><creator>Habel, Ute</creator><creator>Jon Shah, Nadim</creator><creator>Kircher, Tilo</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><general>Oxford University Press</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>PIMPY</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20091101</creationdate><title>Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention</title><author>Thienel, Renate ; Kellermann, Thilo ; Schall, Ulrich ; Voss, Bianca ; Reske, Martina ; Halfter, Sarah ; Sheldrick, Abigail J. ; Radenbach, Katrin ; Habel, Ute ; Jon Shah, Nadim ; Kircher, Tilo</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c559t-f6d17d4a4c3b4ff4a06ccd02db3185268c6f87f70567be48a9fbf3fb05c09a073</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2009</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Attention - drug effects</topic><topic>Attention - physiology</topic><topic>Cross-Over Studies</topic><topic>Executive Function - drug effects</topic><topic>Executive Function - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Muscarinic Antagonists - pharmacology</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - drug effects</topic><topic>Psychomotor Performance - physiology</topic><topic>Reaction Time - drug effects</topic><topic>Reaction Time - physiology</topic><topic>Scopolamine Hydrobromide - pharmacology</topic><topic>Single-Blind Method</topic><topic>Young Adult</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Thienel, Renate</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kellermann, Thilo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Schall, Ulrich</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Voss, Bianca</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Reske, Martina</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Halfter, Sarah</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Sheldrick, Abigail J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Radenbach, Katrin</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Habel, Ute</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Jon Shah, Nadim</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kircher, Tilo</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health &amp; Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health &amp; Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Access via ProQuest (Open Access)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Thienel, Renate</au><au>Kellermann, Thilo</au><au>Schall, Ulrich</au><au>Voss, Bianca</au><au>Reske, Martina</au><au>Halfter, Sarah</au><au>Sheldrick, Abigail J.</au><au>Radenbach, Katrin</au><au>Habel, Ute</au><au>Jon Shah, Nadim</au><au>Kircher, Tilo</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention</atitle><jtitle>The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology</jtitle><addtitle>Int J Neuropsychopharmacol</addtitle><date>2009-11-01</date><risdate>2009</risdate><volume>12</volume><issue>10</issue><spage>1307</spage><epage>1317</epage><pages>1307-1317</pages><issn>1461-1457</issn><eissn>1469-5111</eissn><abstract>Acetylcholine plays a major role in mediating attention processes. We investigated the muscarinic antagonist effect of scopolamine on functional neuro-anatomy of attention and cognition. We assessed 12 healthy volunteers while performing the Attention Network Task on 0.4 mg scopolamine and placebo in a single-blind randomized trial in a 1.5 T magnetic resonance scanner. Neurocognitive measures included verbal learning, verbal memory, verbal fluency, trail making, digit span, a continuous performance task and a planning task (Tower of London). When compared to placebo, scopolamine increased reaction times for conflicting stimulus processing, together with decreasing brain activation in the anterior cingulate cortex (a brain region involved in conflict processing) suggestive of a muscarinic antagonist effect on executive control of attention. Contrary to the notion of a predominantly right-hemispheric lateralization of cognitive processes associated with orienting attention, scopolamine reduced brain activity in left superior and left middle frontal brain areas. Our neuropsychological test data revealed a selective effect of scopolamine on verbal learning and memory while other cognitive domains, such as planning and working memory, were unaffected. These findings are consistent with muscarinic modulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission in frontal attention networks when processing conflicting information.</abstract><cop>Cambridge, UK</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><pmid>19793402</pmid><doi>10.1017/S146114570999068X</doi><tpages>11</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1461-1457
ispartof The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology, 2009-11, Vol.12 (10), p.1307-1317
issn 1461-1457
1469-5111
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_733471406
source MEDLINE; Access via Oxford University Press (Open Access Collection); EZB-FREE-00999 freely available EZB journals
subjects Adult
Attention - drug effects
Attention - physiology
Cross-Over Studies
Executive Function - drug effects
Executive Function - physiology
Humans
Male
Muscarinic Antagonists - pharmacology
Psychomotor Performance - drug effects
Psychomotor Performance - physiology
Reaction Time - drug effects
Reaction Time - physiology
Scopolamine Hydrobromide - pharmacology
Single-Blind Method
Young Adult
title Muscarinic antagonist effects on executive control of attention
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-23T11%3A08%3A47IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Muscarinic%20antagonist%20effects%20on%20executive%20control%20of%20attention&rft.jtitle=The%20international%20journal%20of%20neuropsychopharmacology&rft.au=Thienel,%20Renate&rft.date=2009-11-01&rft.volume=12&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=1307&rft.epage=1317&rft.pages=1307-1317&rft.issn=1461-1457&rft.eissn=1469-5111&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S146114570999068X&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E21146475%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=222360018&rft_id=info:pmid/19793402&rft_cupid=10_1017_S146114570999068X&rfr_iscdi=true