Attribute centrality and imaginative thought
Participants' representations of the concept human were examined to differentiate three types of associations between concepts and their component attributes: the capacity of concepts to cue attributes (attribute accessibility), the capacity of attributes to cue concepts (instance accessibility...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Memory & cognition 2000-12, Vol.28 (8), p.1387-1397 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 1397 |
---|---|
container_issue | 8 |
container_start_page | 1387 |
container_title | Memory & cognition |
container_volume | 28 |
creator | WARD, Thomas B DODDS, Rebecca A SAUNDERS, Katherine N SIFONIS, Cynthia M |
description | Participants' representations of the concept human were examined to differentiate three types of associations between concepts and their component attributes: the capacity of concepts to cue attributes (attribute accessibility), the capacity of attributes to cue concepts (instance accessibility), and the extent to which attributes are thought of as central to concepts (attribute centrality). The findings provide information about the concept human itself and, more generally, about the functionally distinct roles those different attribute-concept associations play in guiding imaginative thought. College students listed attributes that differentiate humans from other animals, rated the centrality of those attributes, and listed animals that possess those attributes. Other students drew and described extraterrestrials that possessed some of the attributes that were found to vary across those listing and rating tasks. Rated centrality was the most important determinant of an attribute's impact on imaginative generation. When the imagined extraterrestrials were supposed to possess attributes that had been rated as central to humans (intelligence, emotional complexity, or opposable thumbs), participants projected more aspects of human form onto them than when the creatures were supposed to possess less central attributes or when attributes were unspecified. |
doi_str_mv | 10.3758/BF03211839 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_72564248</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>69076710</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c402t-9429412f7c9c1e2d4d66ece7a5c9838ca631a2c7bee3fdfabf40be8c17b08b23</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp10E1Lw0AQgOFFFFurF3-ABAUPYnRnv_dYi1Wh4KUHb2Gz2bQpaaK7G6H_3kiLFcHTXB5mhhehc8B3VHJ1_zDFlAAoqg_QEDglKddMHKIhxhqnHJO3AToJYYUx5lyLYzQAIKC1EEN0O47RV3kXXWJdE72pq7hJTFMk1dosqsbE6tMlcdl2i2U8RUelqYM7280Rmk8f55PndPb69DIZz1LLMImpZkQzIKW02oIjBSuEcNZJw61WVFkjKBhiZe4cLYvS5CXDuVMWZI5VTugIXW_Xvvv2o3MhZusqWFfXpnFtFzJJuGCEqR5e_oGrtvNN_1pGQDLGBOgeXf2HQEmJBSZE9upmq6xvQ_CuzN59X8BvMsDZd-VsX7nHF7uVXb52xZ7usv66aYI1delNY6vw45Si0KsvKdCB0w</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877060227</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Attribute centrality and imaginative thought</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerNature Complete Journals</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>WARD, Thomas B ; DODDS, Rebecca A ; SAUNDERS, Katherine N ; SIFONIS, Cynthia M</creator><creatorcontrib>WARD, Thomas B ; DODDS, Rebecca A ; SAUNDERS, Katherine N ; SIFONIS, Cynthia M</creatorcontrib><description>Participants' representations of the concept human were examined to differentiate three types of associations between concepts and their component attributes: the capacity of concepts to cue attributes (attribute accessibility), the capacity of attributes to cue concepts (instance accessibility), and the extent to which attributes are thought of as central to concepts (attribute centrality). The findings provide information about the concept human itself and, more generally, about the functionally distinct roles those different attribute-concept associations play in guiding imaginative thought. College students listed attributes that differentiate humans from other animals, rated the centrality of those attributes, and listed animals that possess those attributes. Other students drew and described extraterrestrials that possessed some of the attributes that were found to vary across those listing and rating tasks. Rated centrality was the most important determinant of an attribute's impact on imaginative generation. When the imagined extraterrestrials were supposed to possess attributes that had been rated as central to humans (intelligence, emotional complexity, or opposable thumbs), participants projected more aspects of human form onto them than when the creatures were supposed to possess less central attributes or when attributes were unspecified.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0090-502X</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1532-5946</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.3758/BF03211839</identifier><identifier>PMID: 11219966</identifier><identifier>CODEN: MYCGAO</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Austin, TX: Psychonomic Society</publisher><subject>Adult ; Biological and medical sciences ; Cognition ; Cognition & reasoning ; Cognition. Intelligence ; College students ; Concept Formation ; Cues ; Emotions ; Female ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Imagination ; Intelligence ; Male ; Mental imagery. Mental representation ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><ispartof>Memory & cognition, 2000-12, Vol.28 (8), p.1387-1397</ispartof><rights>2001 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright Springer Science & Business Media Dec 2000</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c402t-9429412f7c9c1e2d4d66ece7a5c9838ca631a2c7bee3fdfabf40be8c17b08b23</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c402t-9429412f7c9c1e2d4d66ece7a5c9838ca631a2c7bee3fdfabf40be8c17b08b23</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27869,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=883166$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11219966$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>WARD, Thomas B</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>DODDS, Rebecca A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SAUNDERS, Katherine N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SIFONIS, Cynthia M</creatorcontrib><title>Attribute centrality and imaginative thought</title><title>Memory & cognition</title><addtitle>Mem Cognit</addtitle><description>Participants' representations of the concept human were examined to differentiate three types of associations between concepts and their component attributes: the capacity of concepts to cue attributes (attribute accessibility), the capacity of attributes to cue concepts (instance accessibility), and the extent to which attributes are thought of as central to concepts (attribute centrality). The findings provide information about the concept human itself and, more generally, about the functionally distinct roles those different attribute-concept associations play in guiding imaginative thought. College students listed attributes that differentiate humans from other animals, rated the centrality of those attributes, and listed animals that possess those attributes. Other students drew and described extraterrestrials that possessed some of the attributes that were found to vary across those listing and rating tasks. Rated centrality was the most important determinant of an attribute's impact on imaginative generation. When the imagined extraterrestrials were supposed to possess attributes that had been rated as central to humans (intelligence, emotional complexity, or opposable thumbs), participants projected more aspects of human form onto them than when the creatures were supposed to possess less central attributes or when attributes were unspecified.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Cognition</subject><subject>Cognition & reasoning</subject><subject>Cognition. Intelligence</subject><subject>College students</subject><subject>Concept Formation</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Imagination</subject><subject>Intelligence</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Mental imagery. Mental representation</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><issn>0090-502X</issn><issn>1532-5946</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2000</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><recordid>eNp10E1Lw0AQgOFFFFurF3-ABAUPYnRnv_dYi1Wh4KUHb2Gz2bQpaaK7G6H_3kiLFcHTXB5mhhehc8B3VHJ1_zDFlAAoqg_QEDglKddMHKIhxhqnHJO3AToJYYUx5lyLYzQAIKC1EEN0O47RV3kXXWJdE72pq7hJTFMk1dosqsbE6tMlcdl2i2U8RUelqYM7280Rmk8f55PndPb69DIZz1LLMImpZkQzIKW02oIjBSuEcNZJw61WVFkjKBhiZe4cLYvS5CXDuVMWZI5VTugIXW_Xvvv2o3MhZusqWFfXpnFtFzJJuGCEqR5e_oGrtvNN_1pGQDLGBOgeXf2HQEmJBSZE9upmq6xvQ_CuzN59X8BvMsDZd-VsX7nHF7uVXb52xZ7usv66aYI1delNY6vw45Si0KsvKdCB0w</recordid><startdate>20001201</startdate><enddate>20001201</enddate><creator>WARD, Thomas B</creator><creator>DODDS, Rebecca A</creator><creator>SAUNDERS, Katherine N</creator><creator>SIFONIS, Cynthia M</creator><general>Psychonomic Society</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>SDSKB</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>7X8</scope><scope>8BM</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20001201</creationdate><title>Attribute centrality and imaginative thought</title><author>WARD, Thomas B ; DODDS, Rebecca A ; SAUNDERS, Katherine N ; SIFONIS, Cynthia M</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c402t-9429412f7c9c1e2d4d66ece7a5c9838ca631a2c7bee3fdfabf40be8c17b08b23</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2000</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Cognition</topic><topic>Cognition & reasoning</topic><topic>Cognition. Intelligence</topic><topic>College students</topic><topic>Concept Formation</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Imagination</topic><topic>Intelligence</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Mental imagery. Mental representation</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>WARD, Thomas B</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>DODDS, Rebecca A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SAUNDERS, Katherine N</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>SIFONIS, Cynthia M</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 43</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><collection>ComDisDome</collection><jtitle>Memory & cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>WARD, Thomas B</au><au>DODDS, Rebecca A</au><au>SAUNDERS, Katherine N</au><au>SIFONIS, Cynthia M</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Attribute centrality and imaginative thought</atitle><jtitle>Memory & cognition</jtitle><addtitle>Mem Cognit</addtitle><date>2000-12-01</date><risdate>2000</risdate><volume>28</volume><issue>8</issue><spage>1387</spage><epage>1397</epage><pages>1387-1397</pages><issn>0090-502X</issn><eissn>1532-5946</eissn><coden>MYCGAO</coden><abstract>Participants' representations of the concept human were examined to differentiate three types of associations between concepts and their component attributes: the capacity of concepts to cue attributes (attribute accessibility), the capacity of attributes to cue concepts (instance accessibility), and the extent to which attributes are thought of as central to concepts (attribute centrality). The findings provide information about the concept human itself and, more generally, about the functionally distinct roles those different attribute-concept associations play in guiding imaginative thought. College students listed attributes that differentiate humans from other animals, rated the centrality of those attributes, and listed animals that possess those attributes. Other students drew and described extraterrestrials that possessed some of the attributes that were found to vary across those listing and rating tasks. Rated centrality was the most important determinant of an attribute's impact on imaginative generation. When the imagined extraterrestrials were supposed to possess attributes that had been rated as central to humans (intelligence, emotional complexity, or opposable thumbs), participants projected more aspects of human form onto them than when the creatures were supposed to possess less central attributes or when attributes were unspecified.</abstract><cop>Austin, TX</cop><pub>Psychonomic Society</pub><pmid>11219966</pmid><doi>10.3758/BF03211839</doi><tpages>11</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0090-502X |
ispartof | Memory & cognition, 2000-12, Vol.28 (8), p.1387-1397 |
issn | 0090-502X 1532-5946 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_72564248 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerNature Complete Journals; Periodicals Index Online |
subjects | Adult Biological and medical sciences Cognition Cognition & reasoning Cognition. Intelligence College students Concept Formation Cues Emotions Female Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Imagination Intelligence Male Mental imagery. Mental representation Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology |
title | Attribute centrality and imaginative thought |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-28T14%3A49%3A19IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Attribute%20centrality%20and%20imaginative%20thought&rft.jtitle=Memory%20&%20cognition&rft.au=WARD,%20Thomas%20B&rft.date=2000-12-01&rft.volume=28&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=1387&rft.epage=1397&rft.pages=1387-1397&rft.issn=0090-502X&rft.eissn=1532-5946&rft.coden=MYCGAO&rft_id=info:doi/10.3758/BF03211839&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E69076710%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877060227&rft_id=info:pmid/11219966&rfr_iscdi=true |