Spurious Genetic Associations
Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Ge...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Biological psychiatry (1969) 2007-05, Vol.61 (10), p.1121-1126 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 1126 |
---|---|
container_issue | 10 |
container_start_page | 1121 |
container_title | Biological psychiatry (1969) |
container_volume | 61 |
creator | Sullivan, Patrick F |
description | Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_70471930</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0006322306014703</els_id><sourcerecordid>70471930</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkU1r3DAQhkVJaTZpf0JDLunN7oyklexL6RKatBDoIbkLWR5Tbb32VmMH9t9XZrcEeslJDHrmg-cV4gqhREDzeVs2cdzzIfwqJYApEUtAeCNWWFlVSA3yTKwg_xRKSnUuLpi3ubRS4jtxjlZpY2y9Eh8f93OK48zX9zTQFMP1hnkM0U9xHPi9eNv5nunD6b0UT3ffnm6_Fw8_73_cbh6KoCs5FV3rLVgN2HWha7DWlTfgG_SVb4CwqWVtulq3Kq8F3eLakG9BSlOt15lWl-LTcew-jX9m4sntIgfqez9QPs1Z0BZrBa-CWFvURqkMmiMY0sicqHP7FHc-HRyCWwS6rfsn0C0CHaLLAnPj1WnD3OyofWk7GcvAzQnwHHzfJT-EyC9c1r_GauG-HjnK3p4jJcch0hCojYnC5Noxvn7Ll_9GhD4OMW_9TQfi7TinIafi0LF04B6XuJe0wQBqC0r9BazxpIY</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>19714633</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Spurious Genetic Associations</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Sullivan, Patrick F</creator><creatorcontrib>Sullivan, Patrick F</creatorcontrib><description>Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0006-3223</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-2402</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010</identifier><identifier>PMID: 17346679</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BIPCBF</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York, NY: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Algorithms ; Biological and medical sciences ; Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics ; COMT ; Gene Frequency ; genetic association study ; Genotype ; Haplotypes ; Humans ; Linkage Disequilibrium ; Medical sciences ; Models, Genetic ; multiple comparisons ; Phenotype ; Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics ; Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychopathology. Psychiatry ; replication ; Reproducibility of Results ; Schizophrenia - diagnosis ; Schizophrenia - genetics ; Schizophrenic Psychology ; simulation study ; type 1 error</subject><ispartof>Biological psychiatry (1969), 2007-05, Vol.61 (10), p.1121-1126</ispartof><rights>Society of Biological Psychiatry</rights><rights>2007 Society of Biological Psychiatry</rights><rights>2007 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006322306014703$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,27901,27902,65306</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=18735189$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17346679$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Sullivan, Patrick F</creatorcontrib><title>Spurious Genetic Associations</title><title>Biological psychiatry (1969)</title><addtitle>Biol Psychiatry</addtitle><description>Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.</description><subject>Algorithms</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics</subject><subject>COMT</subject><subject>Gene Frequency</subject><subject>genetic association study</subject><subject>Genotype</subject><subject>Haplotypes</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Linkage Disequilibrium</subject><subject>Medical sciences</subject><subject>Models, Genetic</subject><subject>multiple comparisons</subject><subject>Phenotype</subject><subject>Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics</subject><subject>Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</subject><subject>replication</subject><subject>Reproducibility of Results</subject><subject>Schizophrenia - diagnosis</subject><subject>Schizophrenia - genetics</subject><subject>Schizophrenic Psychology</subject><subject>simulation study</subject><subject>type 1 error</subject><issn>0006-3223</issn><issn>1873-2402</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2007</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkU1r3DAQhkVJaTZpf0JDLunN7oyklexL6RKatBDoIbkLWR5Tbb32VmMH9t9XZrcEeslJDHrmg-cV4gqhREDzeVs2cdzzIfwqJYApEUtAeCNWWFlVSA3yTKwg_xRKSnUuLpi3ubRS4jtxjlZpY2y9Eh8f93OK48zX9zTQFMP1hnkM0U9xHPi9eNv5nunD6b0UT3ffnm6_Fw8_73_cbh6KoCs5FV3rLVgN2HWha7DWlTfgG_SVb4CwqWVtulq3Kq8F3eLakG9BSlOt15lWl-LTcew-jX9m4sntIgfqez9QPs1Z0BZrBa-CWFvURqkMmiMY0sicqHP7FHc-HRyCWwS6rfsn0C0CHaLLAnPj1WnD3OyofWk7GcvAzQnwHHzfJT-EyC9c1r_GauG-HjnK3p4jJcch0hCojYnC5Noxvn7Ll_9GhD4OMW_9TQfi7TinIafi0LF04B6XuJe0wQBqC0r9BazxpIY</recordid><startdate>20070515</startdate><enddate>20070515</enddate><creator>Sullivan, Patrick F</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20070515</creationdate><title>Spurious Genetic Associations</title><author>Sullivan, Patrick F</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2007</creationdate><topic>Algorithms</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics</topic><topic>COMT</topic><topic>Gene Frequency</topic><topic>genetic association study</topic><topic>Genotype</topic><topic>Haplotypes</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Linkage Disequilibrium</topic><topic>Medical sciences</topic><topic>Models, Genetic</topic><topic>multiple comparisons</topic><topic>Phenotype</topic><topic>Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics</topic><topic>Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</topic><topic>replication</topic><topic>Reproducibility of Results</topic><topic>Schizophrenia - diagnosis</topic><topic>Schizophrenia - genetics</topic><topic>Schizophrenic Psychology</topic><topic>simulation study</topic><topic>type 1 error</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Sullivan, Patrick F</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Biological psychiatry (1969)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Sullivan, Patrick F</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Spurious Genetic Associations</atitle><jtitle>Biological psychiatry (1969)</jtitle><addtitle>Biol Psychiatry</addtitle><date>2007-05-15</date><risdate>2007</risdate><volume>61</volume><issue>10</issue><spage>1121</spage><epage>1126</epage><pages>1121-1126</pages><issn>0006-3223</issn><eissn>1873-2402</eissn><coden>BIPCBF</coden><abstract>Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.</abstract><cop>New York, NY</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>17346679</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010</doi><tpages>6</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0006-3223 |
ispartof | Biological psychiatry (1969), 2007-05, Vol.61 (10), p.1121-1126 |
issn | 0006-3223 1873-2402 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_70471930 |
source | MEDLINE; Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals |
subjects | Algorithms Biological and medical sciences Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics COMT Gene Frequency genetic association study Genotype Haplotypes Humans Linkage Disequilibrium Medical sciences Models, Genetic multiple comparisons Phenotype Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics Psychiatry Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychopathology. Psychiatry replication Reproducibility of Results Schizophrenia - diagnosis Schizophrenia - genetics Schizophrenic Psychology simulation study type 1 error |
title | Spurious Genetic Associations |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-30T13%3A28%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Spurious%20Genetic%20Associations&rft.jtitle=Biological%20psychiatry%20(1969)&rft.au=Sullivan,%20Patrick%20F&rft.date=2007-05-15&rft.volume=61&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=1121&rft.epage=1126&rft.pages=1121-1126&rft.issn=0006-3223&rft.eissn=1873-2402&rft.coden=BIPCBF&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E70471930%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=19714633&rft_id=info:pmid/17346679&rft_els_id=S0006322306014703&rfr_iscdi=true |