Spurious Genetic Associations

Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Ge...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Biological psychiatry (1969) 2007-05, Vol.61 (10), p.1121-1126
1. Verfasser: Sullivan, Patrick F
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 1126
container_issue 10
container_start_page 1121
container_title Biological psychiatry (1969)
container_volume 61
creator Sullivan, Patrick F
description Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.
doi_str_mv 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_70471930</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0006322306014703</els_id><sourcerecordid>70471930</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkU1r3DAQhkVJaTZpf0JDLunN7oyklexL6RKatBDoIbkLWR5Tbb32VmMH9t9XZrcEeslJDHrmg-cV4gqhREDzeVs2cdzzIfwqJYApEUtAeCNWWFlVSA3yTKwg_xRKSnUuLpi3ubRS4jtxjlZpY2y9Eh8f93OK48zX9zTQFMP1hnkM0U9xHPi9eNv5nunD6b0UT3ffnm6_Fw8_73_cbh6KoCs5FV3rLVgN2HWha7DWlTfgG_SVb4CwqWVtulq3Kq8F3eLakG9BSlOt15lWl-LTcew-jX9m4sntIgfqez9QPs1Z0BZrBa-CWFvURqkMmiMY0sicqHP7FHc-HRyCWwS6rfsn0C0CHaLLAnPj1WnD3OyofWk7GcvAzQnwHHzfJT-EyC9c1r_GauG-HjnK3p4jJcch0hCojYnC5Noxvn7Ll_9GhD4OMW_9TQfi7TinIafi0LF04B6XuJe0wQBqC0r9BazxpIY</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>19714633</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Spurious Genetic Associations</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><creator>Sullivan, Patrick F</creator><creatorcontrib>Sullivan, Patrick F</creatorcontrib><description>Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0006-3223</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1873-2402</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010</identifier><identifier>PMID: 17346679</identifier><identifier>CODEN: BIPCBF</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York, NY: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Algorithms ; Biological and medical sciences ; Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics ; COMT ; Gene Frequency ; genetic association study ; Genotype ; Haplotypes ; Humans ; Linkage Disequilibrium ; Medical sciences ; Models, Genetic ; multiple comparisons ; Phenotype ; Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics ; Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychopathology. Psychiatry ; replication ; Reproducibility of Results ; Schizophrenia - diagnosis ; Schizophrenia - genetics ; Schizophrenic Psychology ; simulation study ; type 1 error</subject><ispartof>Biological psychiatry (1969), 2007-05, Vol.61 (10), p.1121-1126</ispartof><rights>Society of Biological Psychiatry</rights><rights>2007 Society of Biological Psychiatry</rights><rights>2007 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006322306014703$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,3537,27901,27902,65306</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=18735189$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17346679$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Sullivan, Patrick F</creatorcontrib><title>Spurious Genetic Associations</title><title>Biological psychiatry (1969)</title><addtitle>Biol Psychiatry</addtitle><description>Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.</description><subject>Algorithms</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics</subject><subject>COMT</subject><subject>Gene Frequency</subject><subject>genetic association study</subject><subject>Genotype</subject><subject>Haplotypes</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Linkage Disequilibrium</subject><subject>Medical sciences</subject><subject>Models, Genetic</subject><subject>multiple comparisons</subject><subject>Phenotype</subject><subject>Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics</subject><subject>Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</subject><subject>replication</subject><subject>Reproducibility of Results</subject><subject>Schizophrenia - diagnosis</subject><subject>Schizophrenia - genetics</subject><subject>Schizophrenic Psychology</subject><subject>simulation study</subject><subject>type 1 error</subject><issn>0006-3223</issn><issn>1873-2402</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2007</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkU1r3DAQhkVJaTZpf0JDLunN7oyklexL6RKatBDoIbkLWR5Tbb32VmMH9t9XZrcEeslJDHrmg-cV4gqhREDzeVs2cdzzIfwqJYApEUtAeCNWWFlVSA3yTKwg_xRKSnUuLpi3ubRS4jtxjlZpY2y9Eh8f93OK48zX9zTQFMP1hnkM0U9xHPi9eNv5nunD6b0UT3ffnm6_Fw8_73_cbh6KoCs5FV3rLVgN2HWha7DWlTfgG_SVb4CwqWVtulq3Kq8F3eLakG9BSlOt15lWl-LTcew-jX9m4sntIgfqez9QPs1Z0BZrBa-CWFvURqkMmiMY0sicqHP7FHc-HRyCWwS6rfsn0C0CHaLLAnPj1WnD3OyofWk7GcvAzQnwHHzfJT-EyC9c1r_GauG-HjnK3p4jJcch0hCojYnC5Noxvn7Ll_9GhD4OMW_9TQfi7TinIafi0LF04B6XuJe0wQBqC0r9BazxpIY</recordid><startdate>20070515</startdate><enddate>20070515</enddate><creator>Sullivan, Patrick F</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Science</general><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20070515</creationdate><title>Spurious Genetic Associations</title><author>Sullivan, Patrick F</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-fda707401ffcfb1948a60ab1a8ab0e1b9296f94d373404d156ead02268551943</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2007</creationdate><topic>Algorithms</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics</topic><topic>COMT</topic><topic>Gene Frequency</topic><topic>genetic association study</topic><topic>Genotype</topic><topic>Haplotypes</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Linkage Disequilibrium</topic><topic>Medical sciences</topic><topic>Models, Genetic</topic><topic>multiple comparisons</topic><topic>Phenotype</topic><topic>Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics</topic><topic>Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychopathology. Psychiatry</topic><topic>replication</topic><topic>Reproducibility of Results</topic><topic>Schizophrenia - diagnosis</topic><topic>Schizophrenia - genetics</topic><topic>Schizophrenic Psychology</topic><topic>simulation study</topic><topic>type 1 error</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Sullivan, Patrick F</creatorcontrib><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Biological psychiatry (1969)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Sullivan, Patrick F</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Spurious Genetic Associations</atitle><jtitle>Biological psychiatry (1969)</jtitle><addtitle>Biol Psychiatry</addtitle><date>2007-05-15</date><risdate>2007</risdate><volume>61</volume><issue>10</issue><spage>1121</spage><epage>1126</epage><pages>1121-1126</pages><issn>0006-3223</issn><eissn>1873-2402</eissn><coden>BIPCBF</coden><abstract>Background Genetic association studies are widely used in biomedical research and yet only a minority of positive findings stand the test of replication. I explored the capacity of association studies to produce false positive findings and the impact of various definitions of replication. Methods Genetically realistic simulation data of a typical genotyping/analytic approach for 10 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in COMT, a commonly studied candidate gene. Results Candidate gene studies like those simulated here are highly likely to produce one or more false positive findings at α ≤ .05, the pattern of findings can often be “compelling” or “intriguing,” and false positive findings propagate and confuse the literature unless the definition of replication is precise. Conclusions Findings from single association studies constitute “tentative knowledge” and must be interpreted with exceptional caution. For the association method to function as intended, every statistical comparison must be tracked and reported, and integrated replication is essential. Precise replication (the same SNPs, phenotype, and direction of association) is required in the interpretation of multiple association studies.</abstract><cop>New York, NY</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>17346679</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010</doi><tpages>6</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0006-3223
ispartof Biological psychiatry (1969), 2007-05, Vol.61 (10), p.1121-1126
issn 0006-3223
1873-2402
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_70471930
source MEDLINE; Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals
subjects Algorithms
Biological and medical sciences
Catechol O-Methyltransferase - genetics
COMT
Gene Frequency
genetic association study
Genotype
Haplotypes
Humans
Linkage Disequilibrium
Medical sciences
Models, Genetic
multiple comparisons
Phenotype
Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide - genetics
Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychopathology. Psychiatry
replication
Reproducibility of Results
Schizophrenia - diagnosis
Schizophrenia - genetics
Schizophrenic Psychology
simulation study
type 1 error
title Spurious Genetic Associations
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-30T13%3A28%3A55IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Spurious%20Genetic%20Associations&rft.jtitle=Biological%20psychiatry%20(1969)&rft.au=Sullivan,%20Patrick%20F&rft.date=2007-05-15&rft.volume=61&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=1121&rft.epage=1126&rft.pages=1121-1126&rft.issn=0006-3223&rft.eissn=1873-2402&rft.coden=BIPCBF&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.010&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E70471930%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=19714633&rft_id=info:pmid/17346679&rft_els_id=S0006322306014703&rfr_iscdi=true