Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception
Behavioural and functional anatomical responses exhibited by humans support the hypothesis that deception involves the prefrontal executive. Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (among other areas) is activated during lying, compared with telling the...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.) Fla.), 2008-04, Vol.40 (3), p.1411-1418 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 1418 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 1411 |
container_title | NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.) |
container_volume | 40 |
creator | Spence, Sean A. Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine Farrow, Tom F.D. Wilkinson, Iain D. |
description | Behavioural and functional anatomical responses exhibited by humans support the hypothesis that deception involves the prefrontal executive. Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (among other areas) is activated during lying, compared with telling the truth. However, despite some consistencies discernible across studies, problems remain concerning experimental validity, e.g., the expediencies of experimenter-sanctioned cued-deception (i.e., subjects being told when to lie); such ‘lies’ may not have comprised adequate proxies for ‘real-life’ deception. In this experiment, we attempted to address such confounding issues by designing an fMRI paradigm in which subjects chose when to lie (thereby minimising cue-dependency), using spoken words, concerning intimate material, which they regarded as ‘embarrassing’; and where further control conditions required them to ‘comply’ with their examiners or to ‘defy’ them (by withholding pre-specified responses). The main effect of lying revealed significant activation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortices. These results replicate and extend our previous findings to those circumstances under which subjects are allowed to choose when to deceive. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.035 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_70434987</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S1053811908000797</els_id><sourcerecordid>70434987</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c497t-daf6eb4207264e941967d861c8527a1a0cff5e736b5afa357df7024a516d0f7a3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkU2LFDEQhhtR3HX1L0hA8NZtpdP58qaLX7DgwfUcMkn1mrGn0ybpwf33ppmBBS97ShU8b4qqp2kIhY4CFe_23YxriuFg77DrAVQHtAPGnzSXFDRvNZf9063mrFWU6ovmRc57ANB0UM-bC6oYKK7EZeN_LGh_h_mOxJFkdAlLJnb2ZAqY35PbX0hcnEsKu7WEOG_UEWsfJ1sw2YksCcdUiVq6mAr-JSWSY3S19-hw2VIvm2ejnTK-Or9Xzc_Pn26vv7Y33798u_5w07pBy9J6OwrcDT3IXgyoB6qF9EpQp3gvLbXgxpGjZGLH7WgZl36U0A-WU-FhlJZdNW9P_y4p_lkxF3MI2eE02Rnjmo2EgQ1ayUfBnkJPGYgKvvkP3Mc1zXUJQzkIyTTVrFLqRLkUc64HMUuqbtK9oWA2YWZvHoSZTZgBaqqwGn19HrDuDugfgmdDFfh4ArAe7hgwmewCzg59SOiK8TE8PuUf2qms3w</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1506739193</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals</source><source>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</source><creator>Spence, Sean A. ; Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine ; Farrow, Tom F.D. ; Wilkinson, Iain D.</creator><creatorcontrib>Spence, Sean A. ; Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine ; Farrow, Tom F.D. ; Wilkinson, Iain D.</creatorcontrib><description>Behavioural and functional anatomical responses exhibited by humans support the hypothesis that deception involves the prefrontal executive. Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (among other areas) is activated during lying, compared with telling the truth. However, despite some consistencies discernible across studies, problems remain concerning experimental validity, e.g., the expediencies of experimenter-sanctioned cued-deception (i.e., subjects being told when to lie); such ‘lies’ may not have comprised adequate proxies for ‘real-life’ deception. In this experiment, we attempted to address such confounding issues by designing an fMRI paradigm in which subjects chose when to lie (thereby minimising cue-dependency), using spoken words, concerning intimate material, which they regarded as ‘embarrassing’; and where further control conditions required them to ‘comply’ with their examiners or to ‘defy’ them (by withholding pre-specified responses). The main effect of lying revealed significant activation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortices. These results replicate and extend our previous findings to those circumstances under which subjects are allowed to choose when to deceive.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1053-8119</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1095-9572</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.035</identifier><identifier>PMID: 18308586</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Adult ; Behavior ; Brain ; Brain Mapping ; Experiments ; Female ; Humans ; Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ; Lie Detection - psychology ; Magnetic Resonance Imaging ; Male ; Medical imaging ; Middle Aged ; Prefrontal Cortex - physiology ; Studies ; Voice - physiology</subject><ispartof>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 2008-04, Vol.40 (3), p.1411-1418</ispartof><rights>2008 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Copyright Elsevier Limited Apr 15, 2008</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c497t-daf6eb4207264e941967d861c8527a1a0cff5e736b5afa357df7024a516d0f7a3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c497t-daf6eb4207264e941967d861c8527a1a0cff5e736b5afa357df7024a516d0f7a3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.proquest.com/docview/1506739193?pq-origsite=primo$$EHTML$$P50$$Gproquest$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,777,781,3537,27905,27906,45976,64364,64366,64368,72218</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18308586$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Spence, Sean A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Farrow, Tom F.D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wilkinson, Iain D.</creatorcontrib><title>Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception</title><title>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</title><addtitle>Neuroimage</addtitle><description>Behavioural and functional anatomical responses exhibited by humans support the hypothesis that deception involves the prefrontal executive. Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (among other areas) is activated during lying, compared with telling the truth. However, despite some consistencies discernible across studies, problems remain concerning experimental validity, e.g., the expediencies of experimenter-sanctioned cued-deception (i.e., subjects being told when to lie); such ‘lies’ may not have comprised adequate proxies for ‘real-life’ deception. In this experiment, we attempted to address such confounding issues by designing an fMRI paradigm in which subjects chose when to lie (thereby minimising cue-dependency), using spoken words, concerning intimate material, which they regarded as ‘embarrassing’; and where further control conditions required them to ‘comply’ with their examiners or to ‘defy’ them (by withholding pre-specified responses). The main effect of lying revealed significant activation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortices. These results replicate and extend our previous findings to those circumstances under which subjects are allowed to choose when to deceive.</description><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain Mapping</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Image Processing, Computer-Assisted</subject><subject>Lie Detection - psychology</subject><subject>Magnetic Resonance Imaging</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Medical imaging</subject><subject>Middle Aged</subject><subject>Prefrontal Cortex - physiology</subject><subject>Studies</subject><subject>Voice - physiology</subject><issn>1053-8119</issn><issn>1095-9572</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2008</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkU2LFDEQhhtR3HX1L0hA8NZtpdP58qaLX7DgwfUcMkn1mrGn0ybpwf33ppmBBS97ShU8b4qqp2kIhY4CFe_23YxriuFg77DrAVQHtAPGnzSXFDRvNZf9063mrFWU6ovmRc57ANB0UM-bC6oYKK7EZeN_LGh_h_mOxJFkdAlLJnb2ZAqY35PbX0hcnEsKu7WEOG_UEWsfJ1sw2YksCcdUiVq6mAr-JSWSY3S19-hw2VIvm2ejnTK-Or9Xzc_Pn26vv7Y33798u_5w07pBy9J6OwrcDT3IXgyoB6qF9EpQp3gvLbXgxpGjZGLH7WgZl36U0A-WU-FhlJZdNW9P_y4p_lkxF3MI2eE02Rnjmo2EgQ1ayUfBnkJPGYgKvvkP3Mc1zXUJQzkIyTTVrFLqRLkUc64HMUuqbtK9oWA2YWZvHoSZTZgBaqqwGn19HrDuDugfgmdDFfh4ArAe7hgwmewCzg59SOiK8TE8PuUf2qms3w</recordid><startdate>20080415</startdate><enddate>20080415</enddate><creator>Spence, Sean A.</creator><creator>Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine</creator><creator>Farrow, Tom F.D.</creator><creator>Wilkinson, Iain D.</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><general>Elsevier Limited</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FE</scope><scope>8FH</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BBNVY</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BHPHI</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>HCIFZ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>LK8</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M7P</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7QO</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20080415</creationdate><title>Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception</title><author>Spence, Sean A. ; Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine ; Farrow, Tom F.D. ; Wilkinson, Iain D.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c497t-daf6eb4207264e941967d861c8527a1a0cff5e736b5afa357df7024a516d0f7a3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2008</creationdate><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain Mapping</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Image Processing, Computer-Assisted</topic><topic>Lie Detection - psychology</topic><topic>Magnetic Resonance Imaging</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Medical imaging</topic><topic>Middle Aged</topic><topic>Prefrontal Cortex - physiology</topic><topic>Studies</topic><topic>Voice - physiology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Spence, Sean A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Farrow, Tom F.D.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wilkinson, Iain D.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>ProQuest SciTech Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Natural Science Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>SciTech Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Biological Science Collection</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Biological Science Database</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>Biotechnology Research Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Spence, Sean A.</au><au>Kaylor-Hughes, Catherine</au><au>Farrow, Tom F.D.</au><au>Wilkinson, Iain D.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception</atitle><jtitle>NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.)</jtitle><addtitle>Neuroimage</addtitle><date>2008-04-15</date><risdate>2008</risdate><volume>40</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>1411</spage><epage>1418</epage><pages>1411-1418</pages><issn>1053-8119</issn><eissn>1095-9572</eissn><abstract>Behavioural and functional anatomical responses exhibited by humans support the hypothesis that deception involves the prefrontal executive. Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (among other areas) is activated during lying, compared with telling the truth. However, despite some consistencies discernible across studies, problems remain concerning experimental validity, e.g., the expediencies of experimenter-sanctioned cued-deception (i.e., subjects being told when to lie); such ‘lies’ may not have comprised adequate proxies for ‘real-life’ deception. In this experiment, we attempted to address such confounding issues by designing an fMRI paradigm in which subjects chose when to lie (thereby minimising cue-dependency), using spoken words, concerning intimate material, which they regarded as ‘embarrassing’; and where further control conditions required them to ‘comply’ with their examiners or to ‘defy’ them (by withholding pre-specified responses). The main effect of lying revealed significant activation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortices. These results replicate and extend our previous findings to those circumstances under which subjects are allowed to choose when to deceive.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>18308586</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.035</doi><tpages>8</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1053-8119 |
ispartof | NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 2008-04, Vol.40 (3), p.1411-1418 |
issn | 1053-8119 1095-9572 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_70434987 |
source | MEDLINE; Elsevier ScienceDirect Journals; ProQuest Central UK/Ireland |
subjects | Adult Behavior Brain Brain Mapping Experiments Female Humans Image Processing, Computer-Assisted Lie Detection - psychology Magnetic Resonance Imaging Male Medical imaging Middle Aged Prefrontal Cortex - physiology Studies Voice - physiology |
title | Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-18T12%3A38%3A08IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Speaking%20of%20secrets%20and%20lies:%20The%20contribution%20of%20ventrolateral%20prefrontal%20cortex%20to%20vocal%20deception&rft.jtitle=NeuroImage%20(Orlando,%20Fla.)&rft.au=Spence,%20Sean%20A.&rft.date=2008-04-15&rft.volume=40&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=1411&rft.epage=1418&rft.pages=1411-1418&rft.issn=1053-8119&rft.eissn=1095-9572&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.035&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E70434987%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1506739193&rft_id=info:pmid/18308586&rft_els_id=S1053811908000797&rfr_iscdi=true |