Toward a Developmental Model of Child Compliance: The Role of Emotion Regulation in Infancy

The present study examined the relation between early emotion regulation and later compliance. When infants were 5, 10, and 18 months of age, they participated in a frustration task. The degree to which they reacted negatively to the stimuli and the behaviors they used to regulate that response were...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Child development 1999-01, Vol.70 (1), p.21-32
Hauptverfasser: Stifter, Cynthia A., Spinrad, Tracy, Braungart-Rieker, Julia
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 32
container_issue 1
container_start_page 21
container_title Child development
container_volume 70
creator Stifter, Cynthia A.
Spinrad, Tracy
Braungart-Rieker, Julia
description The present study examined the relation between early emotion regulation and later compliance. When infants were 5, 10, and 18 months of age, they participated in a frustration task. The degree to which they reacted negatively to the stimuli and the behaviors they used to regulate that response were coded. Baseline heart rate also was recorded and a measure of cardiac vagal tone (VNA was derived. Several tasks (electrode placement, toy clean-up, and test situation) were administered to elicit compliance/noncompliance when the participants were 30 months of age. Results revealed that infants who demonstrated low levels of regulatory behavior were more likely to be noncompliant as toddlers. Several interaction effects suggested that the prediction to later noncompliance was also dependent upon the infants' level of reactivity. Cardiac vagal tone also was related to compliance but in a contradictory fashion. High VNA was related to noncompliance to toy clean-up, whereas low VNA was related to noncompliance to electrode placement. The data provide support for a developmental model of compliance that includes the ability to regulate emotional arousal.
doi_str_mv 10.1111/1467-8624.00003
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>jstor_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_69673938</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><ericid>EJ591786</ericid><jstor_id>1132012</jstor_id><sourcerecordid>1132012</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a6223-e2026f8065c9a899b584642ff07571b083146403dd61260bfd391a8ece67af033</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkU1v1DAQhi0EokvhzAUhCxC3tP5I_MEN0qW0KlBVCxw4WN7Eod468WInlP33OM1qi5BQ52J73mdGM34BeIrRAU5xiHPGM8FIfoBS0HtgtsvcB7OUkhmVBO2BRzGu0pMwSR-CPYywxAWmM_B94a91qKGGR-aXcX7dmq7XDn70tXHQN7C8tK6GpW_XzuquMm_g4tLAC-_MqM5b31vfwQvzY3D65mo7eNI1Cd08Bg8a7aJ5sj33wZf380X5ITv7fHxSvj3LNCOEZoaksRqBWFFJLaRcFiJnOWkaxAuOl0jQtFOOaF0zTBhaNjWVWAtTGcZ1gyjdB6-nvuvgfw4m9qq1sTLO6c74ISomGaeSijvBgnPJKB47vvgHXPkhdGkJhaVgkggiE_TyvxCjORKcI5Kow4mqgo8xmEatg2112CiM1OihGh1To2PqxsNU8Xzbd1i2pv6Ln0xLwKstoGOlXRPSZ9t4y3GcF3zEnk2YCbbaqfPTQmIuWJLzSb62zmzumkqVR_Ov03TbrqvY-3BbhilBeFw3m2Qbe_N7J-twpZIJvFDfPh2rU0rf8fPzhaL0D0dqz4o</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1634087702</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Toward a Developmental Model of Child Compliance: The Role of Emotion Regulation in Infancy</title><source>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><source>Wiley Online Library Journals【Remote access available】</source><source>MEDLINE</source><source>EBSCOhost Education Source</source><source>JSTOR</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><creator>Stifter, Cynthia A. ; Spinrad, Tracy ; Braungart-Rieker, Julia</creator><creatorcontrib>Stifter, Cynthia A. ; Spinrad, Tracy ; Braungart-Rieker, Julia</creatorcontrib><description>The present study examined the relation between early emotion regulation and later compliance. When infants were 5, 10, and 18 months of age, they participated in a frustration task. The degree to which they reacted negatively to the stimuli and the behaviors they used to regulate that response were coded. Baseline heart rate also was recorded and a measure of cardiac vagal tone (VNA was derived. Several tasks (electrode placement, toy clean-up, and test situation) were administered to elicit compliance/noncompliance when the participants were 30 months of age. Results revealed that infants who demonstrated low levels of regulatory behavior were more likely to be noncompliant as toddlers. Several interaction effects suggested that the prediction to later noncompliance was also dependent upon the infants' level of reactivity. Cardiac vagal tone also was related to compliance but in a contradictory fashion. High VNA was related to noncompliance to toy clean-up, whereas low VNA was related to noncompliance to electrode placement. The data provide support for a developmental model of compliance that includes the ability to regulate emotional arousal.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0009-3920</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1467-8624</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1111/1467-8624.00003</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10191513</identifier><identifier>CODEN: CHDEAW</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Boston, USA and Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers Inc</publisher><subject>Age Factors ; Babies ; Behavior ; Biobehavioral Development, Perception, and Action ; Biological and medical sciences ; Child Behavior - psychology ; Child development ; Child, Preschool ; Children ; Compliance ; Compliance (Psychology) ; Developmental psychology ; Electrocardiography ; Electrodes ; Emotional Development ; Emotional Reactivity ; Emotional Regulation ; Emotions ; Emotions - physiology ; Female ; Frustration ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Heart Rate - physiology ; Humans ; Infancy ; Infant ; Infant Behavior ; Infants ; Longitudinal Studies ; Male ; Models, Psychological ; Newborn. Infant ; Predictor Variables ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Reactivity ; Regulation ; Temperament ; Temperament - physiology ; Toddlers ; Toys ; Vagal Tone</subject><ispartof>Child development, 1999-01, Vol.70 (1), p.21-32</ispartof><rights>Copyright 1999 The Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.</rights><rights>1999 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Copyright Blackwell Publishers Inc. Jan/Feb 1999</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a6223-e2026f8065c9a899b584642ff07571b083146403dd61260bfd391a8ece67af033</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/1132012$$EPDF$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.jstor.org/stable/1132012$$EHTML$$P50$$Gjstor$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,803,1416,27867,27922,27923,30997,30998,45572,45573,58015,58248</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ591786$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&amp;idt=1714573$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10191513$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Stifter, Cynthia A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spinrad, Tracy</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Braungart-Rieker, Julia</creatorcontrib><title>Toward a Developmental Model of Child Compliance: The Role of Emotion Regulation in Infancy</title><title>Child development</title><addtitle>Child Dev</addtitle><description>The present study examined the relation between early emotion regulation and later compliance. When infants were 5, 10, and 18 months of age, they participated in a frustration task. The degree to which they reacted negatively to the stimuli and the behaviors they used to regulate that response were coded. Baseline heart rate also was recorded and a measure of cardiac vagal tone (VNA was derived. Several tasks (electrode placement, toy clean-up, and test situation) were administered to elicit compliance/noncompliance when the participants were 30 months of age. Results revealed that infants who demonstrated low levels of regulatory behavior were more likely to be noncompliant as toddlers. Several interaction effects suggested that the prediction to later noncompliance was also dependent upon the infants' level of reactivity. Cardiac vagal tone also was related to compliance but in a contradictory fashion. High VNA was related to noncompliance to toy clean-up, whereas low VNA was related to noncompliance to electrode placement. The data provide support for a developmental model of compliance that includes the ability to regulate emotional arousal.</description><subject>Age Factors</subject><subject>Babies</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Biobehavioral Development, Perception, and Action</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Child Behavior - psychology</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Child, Preschool</subject><subject>Children</subject><subject>Compliance</subject><subject>Compliance (Psychology)</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Electrocardiography</subject><subject>Electrodes</subject><subject>Emotional Development</subject><subject>Emotional Reactivity</subject><subject>Emotional Regulation</subject><subject>Emotions</subject><subject>Emotions - physiology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Frustration</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Heart Rate - physiology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infancy</subject><subject>Infant</subject><subject>Infant Behavior</subject><subject>Infants</subject><subject>Longitudinal Studies</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Models, Psychological</subject><subject>Newborn. Infant</subject><subject>Predictor Variables</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Reactivity</subject><subject>Regulation</subject><subject>Temperament</subject><subject>Temperament - physiology</subject><subject>Toddlers</subject><subject>Toys</subject><subject>Vagal Tone</subject><issn>0009-3920</issn><issn>1467-8624</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1999</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>K30</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkU1v1DAQhi0EokvhzAUhCxC3tP5I_MEN0qW0KlBVCxw4WN7Eod468WInlP33OM1qi5BQ52J73mdGM34BeIrRAU5xiHPGM8FIfoBS0HtgtsvcB7OUkhmVBO2BRzGu0pMwSR-CPYywxAWmM_B94a91qKGGR-aXcX7dmq7XDn70tXHQN7C8tK6GpW_XzuquMm_g4tLAC-_MqM5b31vfwQvzY3D65mo7eNI1Cd08Bg8a7aJ5sj33wZf380X5ITv7fHxSvj3LNCOEZoaksRqBWFFJLaRcFiJnOWkaxAuOl0jQtFOOaF0zTBhaNjWVWAtTGcZ1gyjdB6-nvuvgfw4m9qq1sTLO6c74ISomGaeSijvBgnPJKB47vvgHXPkhdGkJhaVgkggiE_TyvxCjORKcI5Kow4mqgo8xmEatg2112CiM1OihGh1To2PqxsNU8Xzbd1i2pv6Ln0xLwKstoGOlXRPSZ9t4y3GcF3zEnk2YCbbaqfPTQmIuWJLzSb62zmzumkqVR_Ov03TbrqvY-3BbhilBeFw3m2Qbe_N7J-twpZIJvFDfPh2rU0rf8fPzhaL0D0dqz4o</recordid><startdate>199901</startdate><enddate>199901</enddate><creator>Stifter, Cynthia A.</creator><creator>Spinrad, Tracy</creator><creator>Braungart-Rieker, Julia</creator><general>Blackwell Publishers Inc</general><general>Blackwell Publishers</general><general>Blackwell</general><general>University of Chicago Press for the Society for Research in Child Development, etc</general><general>Blackwell Publishing Ltd</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>FIXVA</scope><scope>FKUCP</scope><scope>IOIBA</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7QJ</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>8BJ</scope><scope>FQK</scope><scope>JBE</scope><scope>U9A</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>199901</creationdate><title>Toward a Developmental Model of Child Compliance: The Role of Emotion Regulation in Infancy</title><author>Stifter, Cynthia A. ; Spinrad, Tracy ; Braungart-Rieker, Julia</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a6223-e2026f8065c9a899b584642ff07571b083146403dd61260bfd391a8ece67af033</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1999</creationdate><topic>Age Factors</topic><topic>Babies</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Biobehavioral Development, Perception, and Action</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Child Behavior - psychology</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Child, Preschool</topic><topic>Children</topic><topic>Compliance</topic><topic>Compliance (Psychology)</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Electrocardiography</topic><topic>Electrodes</topic><topic>Emotional Development</topic><topic>Emotional Reactivity</topic><topic>Emotional Regulation</topic><topic>Emotions</topic><topic>Emotions - physiology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Frustration</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Heart Rate - physiology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infancy</topic><topic>Infant</topic><topic>Infant Behavior</topic><topic>Infants</topic><topic>Longitudinal Studies</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Models, Psychological</topic><topic>Newborn. Infant</topic><topic>Predictor Variables</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Reactivity</topic><topic>Regulation</topic><topic>Temperament</topic><topic>Temperament - physiology</topic><topic>Toddlers</topic><topic>Toys</topic><topic>Vagal Tone</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Stifter, Cynthia A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spinrad, Tracy</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Braungart-Rieker, Julia</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 03</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 04</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 29</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access &amp; Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index &amp; Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences (IBSS)</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>International Bibliography of the Social Sciences</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Child development</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Stifter, Cynthia A.</au><au>Spinrad, Tracy</au><au>Braungart-Rieker, Julia</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ591786</ericid><atitle>Toward a Developmental Model of Child Compliance: The Role of Emotion Regulation in Infancy</atitle><jtitle>Child development</jtitle><addtitle>Child Dev</addtitle><date>1999-01</date><risdate>1999</risdate><volume>70</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>21</spage><epage>32</epage><pages>21-32</pages><issn>0009-3920</issn><eissn>1467-8624</eissn><coden>CHDEAW</coden><abstract>The present study examined the relation between early emotion regulation and later compliance. When infants were 5, 10, and 18 months of age, they participated in a frustration task. The degree to which they reacted negatively to the stimuli and the behaviors they used to regulate that response were coded. Baseline heart rate also was recorded and a measure of cardiac vagal tone (VNA was derived. Several tasks (electrode placement, toy clean-up, and test situation) were administered to elicit compliance/noncompliance when the participants were 30 months of age. Results revealed that infants who demonstrated low levels of regulatory behavior were more likely to be noncompliant as toddlers. Several interaction effects suggested that the prediction to later noncompliance was also dependent upon the infants' level of reactivity. Cardiac vagal tone also was related to compliance but in a contradictory fashion. High VNA was related to noncompliance to toy clean-up, whereas low VNA was related to noncompliance to electrode placement. The data provide support for a developmental model of compliance that includes the ability to regulate emotional arousal.</abstract><cop>Boston, USA and Oxford, UK</cop><pub>Blackwell Publishers Inc</pub><pmid>10191513</pmid><doi>10.1111/1467-8624.00003</doi><tpages>12</tpages></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 0009-3920
ispartof Child development, 1999-01, Vol.70 (1), p.21-32
issn 0009-3920
1467-8624
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_69673938
source Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA); Wiley Online Library Journals【Remote access available】; MEDLINE; EBSCOhost Education Source; JSTOR; Periodicals Index Online
subjects Age Factors
Babies
Behavior
Biobehavioral Development, Perception, and Action
Biological and medical sciences
Child Behavior - psychology
Child development
Child, Preschool
Children
Compliance
Compliance (Psychology)
Developmental psychology
Electrocardiography
Electrodes
Emotional Development
Emotional Reactivity
Emotional Regulation
Emotions
Emotions - physiology
Female
Frustration
Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology
Heart Rate - physiology
Humans
Infancy
Infant
Infant Behavior
Infants
Longitudinal Studies
Male
Models, Psychological
Newborn. Infant
Predictor Variables
Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry
Psychology. Psychophysiology
Reactivity
Regulation
Temperament
Temperament - physiology
Toddlers
Toys
Vagal Tone
title Toward a Developmental Model of Child Compliance: The Role of Emotion Regulation in Infancy
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-14T15%3A42%3A14IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-jstor_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Toward%20a%20Developmental%20Model%20of%20Child%20Compliance:%20The%20Role%20of%20Emotion%20Regulation%20in%20Infancy&rft.jtitle=Child%20development&rft.au=Stifter,%20Cynthia%20A.&rft.date=1999-01&rft.volume=70&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=21&rft.epage=32&rft.pages=21-32&rft.issn=0009-3920&rft.eissn=1467-8624&rft.coden=CHDEAW&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111/1467-8624.00003&rft_dat=%3Cjstor_proqu%3E1132012%3C/jstor_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1634087702&rft_id=info:pmid/10191513&rft_ericid=EJ591786&rft_jstor_id=1132012&rfr_iscdi=true