Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity
Impulsivity is associated with increased risk for alcoholism. Alcohol also may increase impulsive behavior, although little is known about the processes underlying this effect. This study tested a model proposing that the executive processes of working memory (WM) and conditional associative learnin...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Psychopharmacology 1999-10, Vol.146 (4), p.465-472 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 472 |
---|---|
container_issue | 4 |
container_start_page | 465 |
container_title | Psychopharmacology |
container_volume | 146 |
creator | Finn, P R Justus, A Mazas, C Steinmetz, J E |
description | Impulsivity is associated with increased risk for alcoholism. Alcohol also may increase impulsive behavior, although little is known about the processes underlying this effect.
This study tested a model proposing that the executive processes of working memory (WM) and conditional associative learning (CAL) modulate behavioral inhibition. Subjects had either a positive (FHP) or a negative (FHN) family history of alcoholism. Hypotheses were that alcohol would increase Go/No-Go impulsive responding but only in subjects with low working memory capacity (low-WM), low-CAL ability, or FHP for alcoholism. The model also predicted that WM and CAL modulate inhibitory responses to contingency reversal on a Go/No-Go task.
A Go/No-Go learning task with a midway contingency reversal was administered to 71 FHP and 78 FHN subjects when sober and after drinking one of two moderate doses of alcohol. WM (digits backward) and CAL (conditional spatial association task) were also assessed when sober.
Alcohol resulted in more false alarms but only in low-WM subjects. Both WM and CAL modulated learning to inhibit behavior after contingency reversal, suggesting separate modulation mechanisms for WM and CAL. Subjects with low- capacity WM and subjects with low-capacity CAL ability had more difficulty learning response inhibition after contingency reversal. FHPs and FHNs did not differ in their response to alcohol.
The results support our model of the modulatory role of WM and CAL in the ongoing regulation of behavioral inhibitory systems. The results also suggest that individuals with low capacity WM are more susceptible to alcohol's effect of increasing impulsive behavior, suggesting that alcohol reduces the capacity of working memory to modulate response inhibition. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/PL00005492 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_69243583</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>69243583</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c342t-e7ede9f4bfeab06a216c797028013e5633d69b77a09efb5f4118ccd09741d5653</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkcFO3DAQhi0Egu2WCw-ALCFxqBqwYztOuCFEF6RV6aEVx8hxxqypEy92smIfou9cR4sE6qVzmcs3n2bmR-iEkgtKiLz8sSSpBK_yPTSjnOVZTmS-j2aEMJYxKsoj9CnG54niJT9ER5QIQXglZ-jPow-_bf-EO-h82H7F8Ap6HOwG8Dp4DTFCxKpv8bACDMaAHiL2Biun_co77Hu88Jfffbbw2IEKfXJd4QHiMEkV7nwLbhpoYKU21gflcICn0anBptnJbLv16KLd2GH7GR0Y5SIcv_U5-vXt9ufNXbZ8WNzfXC8zzXg-ZCChhcrwxoBqSKFyWmhZSZKXhDIQBWNtUTVSKlKBaYThlJZat6SSnLaiEGyOznfedOPLmJatOxs1OKd68GOsiyrnTJTsvyCVrBRF-vMcnf0DPvsx9OmImlEqpWTp3Yn6sqN08DEGMPU62E6FbU1JPWVZv2eZ4NM35dh00H5Ad-Gxv9uImdg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>3117773497</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals</source><creator>Finn, P R ; Justus, A ; Mazas, C ; Steinmetz, J E</creator><creatorcontrib>Finn, P R ; Justus, A ; Mazas, C ; Steinmetz, J E</creatorcontrib><description>Impulsivity is associated with increased risk for alcoholism. Alcohol also may increase impulsive behavior, although little is known about the processes underlying this effect.
This study tested a model proposing that the executive processes of working memory (WM) and conditional associative learning (CAL) modulate behavioral inhibition. Subjects had either a positive (FHP) or a negative (FHN) family history of alcoholism. Hypotheses were that alcohol would increase Go/No-Go impulsive responding but only in subjects with low working memory capacity (low-WM), low-CAL ability, or FHP for alcoholism. The model also predicted that WM and CAL modulate inhibitory responses to contingency reversal on a Go/No-Go task.
A Go/No-Go learning task with a midway contingency reversal was administered to 71 FHP and 78 FHN subjects when sober and after drinking one of two moderate doses of alcohol. WM (digits backward) and CAL (conditional spatial association task) were also assessed when sober.
Alcohol resulted in more false alarms but only in low-WM subjects. Both WM and CAL modulated learning to inhibit behavior after contingency reversal, suggesting separate modulation mechanisms for WM and CAL. Subjects with low- capacity WM and subjects with low-capacity CAL ability had more difficulty learning response inhibition after contingency reversal. FHPs and FHNs did not differ in their response to alcohol.
The results support our model of the modulatory role of WM and CAL in the ongoing regulation of behavioral inhibitory systems. The results also suggest that individuals with low capacity WM are more susceptible to alcohol's effect of increasing impulsive behavior, suggesting that alcohol reduces the capacity of working memory to modulate response inhibition.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0033-3158</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-2072</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/PL00005492</identifier><identifier>PMID: 10550497</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Germany: Springer Nature B.V</publisher><subject>Adolescent ; Adult ; Alcoholism ; Analysis of Variance ; Associative learning ; Behavior ; Central Nervous System Depressants - administration & dosage ; Central Nervous System Depressants - pharmacology ; Drinking behavior ; Ethanol - administration & dosage ; Ethanol - pharmacology ; Female ; Go/no-go discrimination learning ; Humans ; Impulsive behavior ; Impulsive Behavior - psychology ; Impulsivity ; Inhibition (Psychology) ; Male ; Memory - drug effects ; Reaction Time - drug effects ; Reversal learning</subject><ispartof>Psychopharmacology, 1999-10, Vol.146 (4), p.465-472</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1999.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c342t-e7ede9f4bfeab06a216c797028013e5633d69b77a09efb5f4118ccd09741d5653</citedby></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10550497$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Finn, P R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Justus, A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mazas, C</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steinmetz, J E</creatorcontrib><title>Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity</title><title>Psychopharmacology</title><addtitle>Psychopharmacology (Berl)</addtitle><description>Impulsivity is associated with increased risk for alcoholism. Alcohol also may increase impulsive behavior, although little is known about the processes underlying this effect.
This study tested a model proposing that the executive processes of working memory (WM) and conditional associative learning (CAL) modulate behavioral inhibition. Subjects had either a positive (FHP) or a negative (FHN) family history of alcoholism. Hypotheses were that alcohol would increase Go/No-Go impulsive responding but only in subjects with low working memory capacity (low-WM), low-CAL ability, or FHP for alcoholism. The model also predicted that WM and CAL modulate inhibitory responses to contingency reversal on a Go/No-Go task.
A Go/No-Go learning task with a midway contingency reversal was administered to 71 FHP and 78 FHN subjects when sober and after drinking one of two moderate doses of alcohol. WM (digits backward) and CAL (conditional spatial association task) were also assessed when sober.
Alcohol resulted in more false alarms but only in low-WM subjects. Both WM and CAL modulated learning to inhibit behavior after contingency reversal, suggesting separate modulation mechanisms for WM and CAL. Subjects with low- capacity WM and subjects with low-capacity CAL ability had more difficulty learning response inhibition after contingency reversal. FHPs and FHNs did not differ in their response to alcohol.
The results support our model of the modulatory role of WM and CAL in the ongoing regulation of behavioral inhibitory systems. The results also suggest that individuals with low capacity WM are more susceptible to alcohol's effect of increasing impulsive behavior, suggesting that alcohol reduces the capacity of working memory to modulate response inhibition.</description><subject>Adolescent</subject><subject>Adult</subject><subject>Alcoholism</subject><subject>Analysis of Variance</subject><subject>Associative learning</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Central Nervous System Depressants - administration & dosage</subject><subject>Central Nervous System Depressants - pharmacology</subject><subject>Drinking behavior</subject><subject>Ethanol - administration & dosage</subject><subject>Ethanol - pharmacology</subject><subject>Female</subject><subject>Go/no-go discrimination learning</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Impulsive behavior</subject><subject>Impulsive Behavior - psychology</subject><subject>Impulsivity</subject><subject>Inhibition (Psychology)</subject><subject>Male</subject><subject>Memory - drug effects</subject><subject>Reaction Time - drug effects</subject><subject>Reversal learning</subject><issn>0033-3158</issn><issn>1432-2072</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1999</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkcFO3DAQhi0Egu2WCw-ALCFxqBqwYztOuCFEF6RV6aEVx8hxxqypEy92smIfou9cR4sE6qVzmcs3n2bmR-iEkgtKiLz8sSSpBK_yPTSjnOVZTmS-j2aEMJYxKsoj9CnG54niJT9ER5QIQXglZ-jPow-_bf-EO-h82H7F8Ap6HOwG8Dp4DTFCxKpv8bACDMaAHiL2Biun_co77Hu88Jfffbbw2IEKfXJd4QHiMEkV7nwLbhpoYKU21gflcICn0anBptnJbLv16KLd2GH7GR0Y5SIcv_U5-vXt9ufNXbZ8WNzfXC8zzXg-ZCChhcrwxoBqSKFyWmhZSZKXhDIQBWNtUTVSKlKBaYThlJZat6SSnLaiEGyOznfedOPLmJatOxs1OKd68GOsiyrnTJTsvyCVrBRF-vMcnf0DPvsx9OmImlEqpWTp3Yn6sqN08DEGMPU62E6FbU1JPWVZv2eZ4NM35dh00H5Ad-Gxv9uImdg</recordid><startdate>19991001</startdate><enddate>19991001</enddate><creator>Finn, P R</creator><creator>Justus, A</creator><creator>Mazas, C</creator><creator>Steinmetz, J E</creator><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>19991001</creationdate><title>Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity</title><author>Finn, P R ; Justus, A ; Mazas, C ; Steinmetz, J E</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c342t-e7ede9f4bfeab06a216c797028013e5633d69b77a09efb5f4118ccd09741d5653</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1999</creationdate><topic>Adolescent</topic><topic>Adult</topic><topic>Alcoholism</topic><topic>Analysis of Variance</topic><topic>Associative learning</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Central Nervous System Depressants - administration & dosage</topic><topic>Central Nervous System Depressants - pharmacology</topic><topic>Drinking behavior</topic><topic>Ethanol - administration & dosage</topic><topic>Ethanol - pharmacology</topic><topic>Female</topic><topic>Go/no-go discrimination learning</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Impulsive behavior</topic><topic>Impulsive Behavior - psychology</topic><topic>Impulsivity</topic><topic>Inhibition (Psychology)</topic><topic>Male</topic><topic>Memory - drug effects</topic><topic>Reaction Time - drug effects</topic><topic>Reversal learning</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Finn, P R</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Justus, A</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Mazas, C</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Steinmetz, J E</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Psychology</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Psychopharmacology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Finn, P R</au><au>Justus, A</au><au>Mazas, C</au><au>Steinmetz, J E</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity</atitle><jtitle>Psychopharmacology</jtitle><addtitle>Psychopharmacology (Berl)</addtitle><date>1999-10-01</date><risdate>1999</risdate><volume>146</volume><issue>4</issue><spage>465</spage><epage>472</epage><pages>465-472</pages><issn>0033-3158</issn><eissn>1432-2072</eissn><abstract>Impulsivity is associated with increased risk for alcoholism. Alcohol also may increase impulsive behavior, although little is known about the processes underlying this effect.
This study tested a model proposing that the executive processes of working memory (WM) and conditional associative learning (CAL) modulate behavioral inhibition. Subjects had either a positive (FHP) or a negative (FHN) family history of alcoholism. Hypotheses were that alcohol would increase Go/No-Go impulsive responding but only in subjects with low working memory capacity (low-WM), low-CAL ability, or FHP for alcoholism. The model also predicted that WM and CAL modulate inhibitory responses to contingency reversal on a Go/No-Go task.
A Go/No-Go learning task with a midway contingency reversal was administered to 71 FHP and 78 FHN subjects when sober and after drinking one of two moderate doses of alcohol. WM (digits backward) and CAL (conditional spatial association task) were also assessed when sober.
Alcohol resulted in more false alarms but only in low-WM subjects. Both WM and CAL modulated learning to inhibit behavior after contingency reversal, suggesting separate modulation mechanisms for WM and CAL. Subjects with low- capacity WM and subjects with low-capacity CAL ability had more difficulty learning response inhibition after contingency reversal. FHPs and FHNs did not differ in their response to alcohol.
The results support our model of the modulatory role of WM and CAL in the ongoing regulation of behavioral inhibitory systems. The results also suggest that individuals with low capacity WM are more susceptible to alcohol's effect of increasing impulsive behavior, suggesting that alcohol reduces the capacity of working memory to modulate response inhibition.</abstract><cop>Germany</cop><pub>Springer Nature B.V</pub><pmid>10550497</pmid><doi>10.1007/PL00005492</doi><tpages>8</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0033-3158 |
ispartof | Psychopharmacology, 1999-10, Vol.146 (4), p.465-472 |
issn | 0033-3158 1432-2072 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_69243583 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals |
subjects | Adolescent Adult Alcoholism Analysis of Variance Associative learning Behavior Central Nervous System Depressants - administration & dosage Central Nervous System Depressants - pharmacology Drinking behavior Ethanol - administration & dosage Ethanol - pharmacology Female Go/no-go discrimination learning Humans Impulsive behavior Impulsive Behavior - psychology Impulsivity Inhibition (Psychology) Male Memory - drug effects Reaction Time - drug effects Reversal learning |
title | Working memory, executive processes and the effects of alcohol on Go/No-Go learning: testing a model of behavioral regulation and impulsivity |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-05T03%3A12%3A51IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Working%20memory,%20executive%20processes%20and%20the%20effects%20of%20alcohol%20on%20Go/No-Go%20learning:%20testing%20a%20model%20of%20behavioral%20regulation%20and%20impulsivity&rft.jtitle=Psychopharmacology&rft.au=Finn,%20P%20R&rft.date=1999-10-01&rft.volume=146&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=465&rft.epage=472&rft.pages=465-472&rft.issn=0033-3158&rft.eissn=1432-2072&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/PL00005492&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E69243583%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=3117773497&rft_id=info:pmid/10550497&rfr_iscdi=true |