Reflective thinking and mental imagery: A perspective on the development of posttraumatic stress disorder
Reflective thinking occurs when information stored in long-term memory (LTM) is not sufficient to allow one to respond “automatically” to an object or event. Instead, stored information must be entered into working memory and a novel response or solution produced. In this article I argue that mental...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Development and psychopathology 2005-09, Vol.17 (3), p.851-863 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 863 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | 851 |
container_title | Development and psychopathology |
container_volume | 17 |
creator | KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M. |
description | Reflective thinking occurs when information stored in long-term memory
(LTM) is not sufficient to allow one to respond
“automatically” to an object or event. Instead, stored
information must be entered into working memory and a novel response or
solution produced. In this article I argue that mental imagery plays a
central role in this process, and that over the course of normal cognitive
development the process of reflective thinking “programs” LTM
so that an increasingly large number of tasks can be performed without
reflective thinking. Normal cognitive development thus results in a
decreasing reliance on imagery. However, if highly emotional images are
formed, additional retrieval cues can be entered into LTM, making such
images more likely to occur in the future. Such images induce arousal,
similar to that induced by the actual event. This line of thinking leads
to a novel perspective on the neurocognitive deficits that underlie the
development of posttraumatic stress disorder, and may also help to explain
some symptoms seen in hyperactivity, impulsiveness, and difficulties in
self-control.The work summarized in this
paper was supported in part by NSF ROLE Grant REC-0106760 and NIH Grant 5
R01 MH60734. Portions of this article were adapted from a draft of Kosslyn
(2005). I thank the Editors and Robin Rosenberg
for helpful comments on earlier drafts. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1017/S0954579405050406 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_68752579</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><cupid>10_1017_S0954579405050406</cupid><sourcerecordid>68752579</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-a558t-10e19d20f7c224f74f689afae61ad0d141ebc352059f51ee9abce50d9338874f3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqF0c1rFDEUAPAgit1W_wAvEjx4G833TLyVxXaFBVEregvZyZs17cxkTDLF_vfNsoMFRSSHHN7vPd4HQi8oeUMJrd9-IVoKWWtBZHmCqEdoRYXSFaO6eYxWh3B1iJ-g05SuCSGSC_kUnVDFFNNarpD_DF0Pbfa3gPMPP974cY_t6PAAY7Y99oPdQ7x7h8_xBDFNCw1j0YAd3EIfpoPFocNTSDlHOw82-xanHCEl7HwK0UF8hp50tk_wfPnP0NeL91frTbX9ePlhfb6trJRNrigBqh0jXd0yJrpadKrRtrOgqHXEUUFh13LJiNSdpADa7lqQxGnOm6ZofoZeH-tOMfycIWUz-NRC39sRwpyMamrJylL-C6nmgjLeFPjqD3gd5jiWIQwjVOmaNLIgekRtDClF6MwUy_LinaHEHK5l_rpWyXm5FJ53A7iHjOU8BVRH4FOGX7_jNt4YVfNaGnX5yWy23y_k1beNWRfPlybssIve7eGh1X-3cQ9ssq7W</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>201697085</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Reflective thinking and mental imagery: A perspective on the development of posttraumatic stress disorder</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Cambridge Journals</source><creator>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</creator><creatorcontrib>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</creatorcontrib><description>Reflective thinking occurs when information stored in long-term memory
(LTM) is not sufficient to allow one to respond
“automatically” to an object or event. Instead, stored
information must be entered into working memory and a novel response or
solution produced. In this article I argue that mental imagery plays a
central role in this process, and that over the course of normal cognitive
development the process of reflective thinking “programs” LTM
so that an increasingly large number of tasks can be performed without
reflective thinking. Normal cognitive development thus results in a
decreasing reliance on imagery. However, if highly emotional images are
formed, additional retrieval cues can be entered into LTM, making such
images more likely to occur in the future. Such images induce arousal,
similar to that induced by the actual event. This line of thinking leads
to a novel perspective on the neurocognitive deficits that underlie the
development of posttraumatic stress disorder, and may also help to explain
some symptoms seen in hyperactivity, impulsiveness, and difficulties in
self-control.The work summarized in this
paper was supported in part by NSF ROLE Grant REC-0106760 and NIH Grant 5
R01 MH60734. Portions of this article were adapted from a draft of Kosslyn
(2005). I thank the Editors and Robin Rosenberg
for helpful comments on earlier drafts.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0954-5794</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1469-2198</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1017/S0954579405050406</identifier><identifier>PMID: 16262995</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>New York, USA: Cambridge University Press</publisher><subject>Brain ; Brain - physiopathology ; Children & youth ; Cognition - physiology ; Developmental psychology ; Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders - psychology ; Humans ; Imagination ; Memory ; Post traumatic stress disorder ; Social Control, Informal ; Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - physiopathology ; Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - psychology ; Thinking</subject><ispartof>Development and psychopathology, 2005-09, Vol.17 (3), p.851-863</ispartof><rights>2005 Cambridge University Press</rights><rights>Copyright Cambridge University Press, Publishing Division Jul 2005</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-a558t-10e19d20f7c224f74f689afae61ad0d141ebc352059f51ee9abce50d9338874f3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-a558t-10e19d20f7c224f74f689afae61ad0d141ebc352059f51ee9abce50d9338874f3</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0954579405050406/type/journal_article$$EHTML$$P50$$Gcambridge$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>164,314,780,784,27923,27924,55627</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16262995$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</creatorcontrib><title>Reflective thinking and mental imagery: A perspective on the development of posttraumatic stress disorder</title><title>Development and psychopathology</title><addtitle>Dev Psychopathol</addtitle><description>Reflective thinking occurs when information stored in long-term memory
(LTM) is not sufficient to allow one to respond
“automatically” to an object or event. Instead, stored
information must be entered into working memory and a novel response or
solution produced. In this article I argue that mental imagery plays a
central role in this process, and that over the course of normal cognitive
development the process of reflective thinking “programs” LTM
so that an increasingly large number of tasks can be performed without
reflective thinking. Normal cognitive development thus results in a
decreasing reliance on imagery. However, if highly emotional images are
formed, additional retrieval cues can be entered into LTM, making such
images more likely to occur in the future. Such images induce arousal,
similar to that induced by the actual event. This line of thinking leads
to a novel perspective on the neurocognitive deficits that underlie the
development of posttraumatic stress disorder, and may also help to explain
some symptoms seen in hyperactivity, impulsiveness, and difficulties in
self-control.The work summarized in this
paper was supported in part by NSF ROLE Grant REC-0106760 and NIH Grant 5
R01 MH60734. Portions of this article were adapted from a draft of Kosslyn
(2005). I thank the Editors and Robin Rosenberg
for helpful comments on earlier drafts.</description><subject>Brain</subject><subject>Brain - physiopathology</subject><subject>Children & youth</subject><subject>Cognition - physiology</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders - psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Imagination</subject><subject>Memory</subject><subject>Post traumatic stress disorder</subject><subject>Social Control, Informal</subject><subject>Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - physiopathology</subject><subject>Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - psychology</subject><subject>Thinking</subject><issn>0954-5794</issn><issn>1469-2198</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2005</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNqF0c1rFDEUAPAgit1W_wAvEjx4G833TLyVxXaFBVEregvZyZs17cxkTDLF_vfNsoMFRSSHHN7vPd4HQi8oeUMJrd9-IVoKWWtBZHmCqEdoRYXSFaO6eYxWh3B1iJ-g05SuCSGSC_kUnVDFFNNarpD_DF0Pbfa3gPMPP974cY_t6PAAY7Y99oPdQ7x7h8_xBDFNCw1j0YAd3EIfpoPFocNTSDlHOw82-xanHCEl7HwK0UF8hp50tk_wfPnP0NeL91frTbX9ePlhfb6trJRNrigBqh0jXd0yJrpadKrRtrOgqHXEUUFh13LJiNSdpADa7lqQxGnOm6ZofoZeH-tOMfycIWUz-NRC39sRwpyMamrJylL-C6nmgjLeFPjqD3gd5jiWIQwjVOmaNLIgekRtDClF6MwUy_LinaHEHK5l_rpWyXm5FJ53A7iHjOU8BVRH4FOGX7_jNt4YVfNaGnX5yWy23y_k1beNWRfPlybssIve7eGh1X-3cQ9ssq7W</recordid><startdate>20050901</startdate><enddate>20050901</enddate><creator>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</creator><general>Cambridge University Press</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8AM</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AN0</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>BGRYB</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K7.</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0O</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20050901</creationdate><title>Reflective thinking and mental imagery: A perspective on the development of posttraumatic stress disorder</title><author>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-a558t-10e19d20f7c224f74f689afae61ad0d141ebc352059f51ee9abce50d9338874f3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2005</creationdate><topic>Brain</topic><topic>Brain - physiopathology</topic><topic>Children & youth</topic><topic>Cognition - physiology</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders - psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Imagination</topic><topic>Memory</topic><topic>Post traumatic stress disorder</topic><topic>Social Control, Informal</topic><topic>Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - physiopathology</topic><topic>Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - psychology</topic><topic>Thinking</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Criminal Justice Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>British Nursing Database</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Criminology Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Criminal Justice (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Criminal Justice Database</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Development and psychopathology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>KOSSLYN, STEPHEN M.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Reflective thinking and mental imagery: A perspective on the development of posttraumatic stress disorder</atitle><jtitle>Development and psychopathology</jtitle><addtitle>Dev Psychopathol</addtitle><date>2005-09-01</date><risdate>2005</risdate><volume>17</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>851</spage><epage>863</epage><pages>851-863</pages><issn>0954-5794</issn><eissn>1469-2198</eissn><abstract>Reflective thinking occurs when information stored in long-term memory
(LTM) is not sufficient to allow one to respond
“automatically” to an object or event. Instead, stored
information must be entered into working memory and a novel response or
solution produced. In this article I argue that mental imagery plays a
central role in this process, and that over the course of normal cognitive
development the process of reflective thinking “programs” LTM
so that an increasingly large number of tasks can be performed without
reflective thinking. Normal cognitive development thus results in a
decreasing reliance on imagery. However, if highly emotional images are
formed, additional retrieval cues can be entered into LTM, making such
images more likely to occur in the future. Such images induce arousal,
similar to that induced by the actual event. This line of thinking leads
to a novel perspective on the neurocognitive deficits that underlie the
development of posttraumatic stress disorder, and may also help to explain
some symptoms seen in hyperactivity, impulsiveness, and difficulties in
self-control.The work summarized in this
paper was supported in part by NSF ROLE Grant REC-0106760 and NIH Grant 5
R01 MH60734. Portions of this article were adapted from a draft of Kosslyn
(2005). I thank the Editors and Robin Rosenberg
for helpful comments on earlier drafts.</abstract><cop>New York, USA</cop><pub>Cambridge University Press</pub><pmid>16262995</pmid><doi>10.1017/S0954579405050406</doi><tpages>13</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0954-5794 |
ispartof | Development and psychopathology, 2005-09, Vol.17 (3), p.851-863 |
issn | 0954-5794 1469-2198 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_68752579 |
source | MEDLINE; Cambridge Journals |
subjects | Brain Brain - physiopathology Children & youth Cognition - physiology Developmental psychology Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders - psychology Humans Imagination Memory Post traumatic stress disorder Social Control, Informal Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - physiopathology Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic - psychology Thinking |
title | Reflective thinking and mental imagery: A perspective on the development of posttraumatic stress disorder |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-11T07%3A17%3A17IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Reflective%20thinking%20and%20mental%20imagery:%20A%20perspective%20on%20the%20development%20of%20posttraumatic%20stress%20disorder&rft.jtitle=Development%20and%20psychopathology&rft.au=KOSSLYN,%20STEPHEN%20M.&rft.date=2005-09-01&rft.volume=17&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=851&rft.epage=863&rft.pages=851-863&rft.issn=0954-5794&rft.eissn=1469-2198&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017/S0954579405050406&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E68752579%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=201697085&rft_id=info:pmid/16262995&rft_cupid=10_1017_S0954579405050406&rfr_iscdi=true |