New insights into action-perception coupling
According to a view that has dominated the field for over a century, the brain programs muscle commands and uses a copy of these commands [efference copy (EC)] to adjust not only resulting motor action but also ongoing perception. This view was helpful in formulating several classical problems of ac...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Experimental brain research 2009-03, Vol.194 (1), p.39-58 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 58 |
---|---|
container_issue | 1 |
container_start_page | 39 |
container_title | Experimental brain research |
container_volume | 194 |
creator | Feldman, Anatol G |
description | According to a view that has dominated the field for over a century, the brain programs muscle commands and uses a copy of these commands [efference copy (EC)] to adjust not only resulting motor action but also ongoing perception. This view was helpful in formulating several classical problems of action and perception: (1) the posture-movement problem of how movements away from a stable posture can be made without evoking resistance of posture-stabilizing mechanisms resulting from intrinsic muscle and reflex properties; (2) the problem of kinesthesia or why our sense of limb position is good despite ambiguous positional information delivered by proprioceptive and cutaneous signals; (3) the problem of visual space constancy or why the world is perceived as stable while its retinal image shifts following changes in gaze. On closer inspection, the EC theory actually does not solve these problems in a physiologically feasible way. Here solutions to these problems are proposed based on the advanced formulation of the equilibrium-point hypothesis that suggests that action and perception are accomplished in a common spatial frame of reference selected by the brain from a set of available frames. Experimental data suggest that the brain is also able to translate or/and rotate the selected frame of reference by modifying its major attributes--the origin, metrics and orientation--and thus substantially influence action and perception. Because of this ability, such frames are called physical to distinguish them from symbolic or mathematical frames that are used to describe system behavior without influencing this behavior. Experimental data also imply that once a frame of reference is chosen, its attributes are modified in a feedforward way, thus enabling the brain to act in an anticipatory and predictive manner. This approach is extended to sense of effort, kinesthetic illusions, phantom limb and phantom body phenomena. It also addresses the question of why retinal images of objects are sensed as objects located in the external, physical world, rather than in internal representations of the brain. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_67012504</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1658884581</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c454t-480efb5d3832ebd70fcd2c1d9fa890e19b0da2cb55d5d2d4405d5e408a40dac83</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkE9P3DAQxa0KVBbaD9ALXVWCE2lnxnbiHBECioTgQDlbju1sg7JJsBMhvn0dZVUkDu3Jz5rfmz-PsS8I3xGg-BEBiDADUBnmeZHxD2yFglOGCPkeWwGgyITC8oAdxvg0f3kBH9kBlqBIEa7Y2Z1_WTddbDa_x5jE2K-NHZu-ywYfrB9mubb9NLRNt_nE9mvTRv959x6xx6vLXxc_s9v765uL89vMCinGNBF8XUnHFSdfuQJq68iiK2ujSvBYVuAM2UpKJx05ISAJL0AZkQpW8SN2uvQdQv88-TjqbROtb1vT-X6KOi8ASYL4L0ggkfMCE_jtHfjUT6FLR2jCxAjFeYJwgWzoYwy-1kNotia8agQ9B66XwHUKXM-B69lzvGs8VVvv3hy7hBNwsgNMtKatg-lsE_9yhMRlLudTaOFiKnUbH942_Nf0r4upNr02m5AaPz4QIAfMQSoi_gdO6Z99</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>215134833</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>New insights into action-perception coupling</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings</source><creator>Feldman, Anatol G</creator><creatorcontrib>Feldman, Anatol G</creatorcontrib><description>According to a view that has dominated the field for over a century, the brain programs muscle commands and uses a copy of these commands [efference copy (EC)] to adjust not only resulting motor action but also ongoing perception. This view was helpful in formulating several classical problems of action and perception: (1) the posture-movement problem of how movements away from a stable posture can be made without evoking resistance of posture-stabilizing mechanisms resulting from intrinsic muscle and reflex properties; (2) the problem of kinesthesia or why our sense of limb position is good despite ambiguous positional information delivered by proprioceptive and cutaneous signals; (3) the problem of visual space constancy or why the world is perceived as stable while its retinal image shifts following changes in gaze. On closer inspection, the EC theory actually does not solve these problems in a physiologically feasible way. Here solutions to these problems are proposed based on the advanced formulation of the equilibrium-point hypothesis that suggests that action and perception are accomplished in a common spatial frame of reference selected by the brain from a set of available frames. Experimental data suggest that the brain is also able to translate or/and rotate the selected frame of reference by modifying its major attributes--the origin, metrics and orientation--and thus substantially influence action and perception. Because of this ability, such frames are called physical to distinguish them from symbolic or mathematical frames that are used to describe system behavior without influencing this behavior. Experimental data also imply that once a frame of reference is chosen, its attributes are modified in a feedforward way, thus enabling the brain to act in an anticipatory and predictive manner. This approach is extended to sense of effort, kinesthetic illusions, phantom limb and phantom body phenomena. It also addresses the question of why retinal images of objects are sensed as objects located in the external, physical world, rather than in internal representations of the brain.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0014-4819</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1432-1106</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3</identifier><identifier>PMID: 19082821</identifier><identifier>CODEN: EXBRAP</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Berlin/Heidelberg : Springer-Verlag</publisher><subject>Algorithms ; Ambiguity ; Animals ; Biological and medical sciences ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Biomedicine ; Brain - physiology ; Brain research ; Child development ; Developmental psychology ; Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision ; Eye Movements ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Humans ; Illusions ; Models, Neurological ; Motor Activity ; Motor Neurons - physiology ; Muscle, Skeletal - physiology ; Neurology ; Neurosciences ; Newborn. Infant ; Perception ; Perceptions ; Phantom Limb ; Postural Balance ; Posture ; Proprioception ; Proprioception - physiology ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Research Article ; Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><ispartof>Experimental brain research, 2009-03, Vol.194 (1), p.39-58</ispartof><rights>Springer-Verlag 2008</rights><rights>2009 INIST-CNRS</rights><rights>Springer-Verlag 2009</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c454t-480efb5d3832ebd70fcd2c1d9fa890e19b0da2cb55d5d2d4405d5e408a40dac83</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c454t-480efb5d3832ebd70fcd2c1d9fa890e19b0da2cb55d5d2d4405d5e408a40dac83</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27923,27924,41487,42556,51318</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=21235654$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19082821$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Feldman, Anatol G</creatorcontrib><title>New insights into action-perception coupling</title><title>Experimental brain research</title><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><description>According to a view that has dominated the field for over a century, the brain programs muscle commands and uses a copy of these commands [efference copy (EC)] to adjust not only resulting motor action but also ongoing perception. This view was helpful in formulating several classical problems of action and perception: (1) the posture-movement problem of how movements away from a stable posture can be made without evoking resistance of posture-stabilizing mechanisms resulting from intrinsic muscle and reflex properties; (2) the problem of kinesthesia or why our sense of limb position is good despite ambiguous positional information delivered by proprioceptive and cutaneous signals; (3) the problem of visual space constancy or why the world is perceived as stable while its retinal image shifts following changes in gaze. On closer inspection, the EC theory actually does not solve these problems in a physiologically feasible way. Here solutions to these problems are proposed based on the advanced formulation of the equilibrium-point hypothesis that suggests that action and perception are accomplished in a common spatial frame of reference selected by the brain from a set of available frames. Experimental data suggest that the brain is also able to translate or/and rotate the selected frame of reference by modifying its major attributes--the origin, metrics and orientation--and thus substantially influence action and perception. Because of this ability, such frames are called physical to distinguish them from symbolic or mathematical frames that are used to describe system behavior without influencing this behavior. Experimental data also imply that once a frame of reference is chosen, its attributes are modified in a feedforward way, thus enabling the brain to act in an anticipatory and predictive manner. This approach is extended to sense of effort, kinesthetic illusions, phantom limb and phantom body phenomena. It also addresses the question of why retinal images of objects are sensed as objects located in the external, physical world, rather than in internal representations of the brain.</description><subject>Algorithms</subject><subject>Ambiguity</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedicine</subject><subject>Brain - physiology</subject><subject>Brain research</subject><subject>Child development</subject><subject>Developmental psychology</subject><subject>Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision</subject><subject>Eye Movements</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Illusions</subject><subject>Models, Neurological</subject><subject>Motor Activity</subject><subject>Motor Neurons - physiology</subject><subject>Muscle, Skeletal - physiology</subject><subject>Neurology</subject><subject>Neurosciences</subject><subject>Newborn. Infant</subject><subject>Perception</subject><subject>Perceptions</subject><subject>Phantom Limb</subject><subject>Postural Balance</subject><subject>Posture</subject><subject>Proprioception</subject><subject>Proprioception - physiology</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Research Article</subject><subject>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</subject><issn>0014-4819</issn><issn>1432-1106</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2009</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkE9P3DAQxa0KVBbaD9ALXVWCE2lnxnbiHBECioTgQDlbju1sg7JJsBMhvn0dZVUkDu3Jz5rfmz-PsS8I3xGg-BEBiDADUBnmeZHxD2yFglOGCPkeWwGgyITC8oAdxvg0f3kBH9kBlqBIEa7Y2Z1_WTddbDa_x5jE2K-NHZu-ywYfrB9mubb9NLRNt_nE9mvTRv959x6xx6vLXxc_s9v765uL89vMCinGNBF8XUnHFSdfuQJq68iiK2ujSvBYVuAM2UpKJx05ISAJL0AZkQpW8SN2uvQdQv88-TjqbROtb1vT-X6KOi8ASYL4L0ggkfMCE_jtHfjUT6FLR2jCxAjFeYJwgWzoYwy-1kNotia8agQ9B66XwHUKXM-B69lzvGs8VVvv3hy7hBNwsgNMtKatg-lsE_9yhMRlLudTaOFiKnUbH942_Nf0r4upNr02m5AaPz4QIAfMQSoi_gdO6Z99</recordid><startdate>20090301</startdate><enddate>20090301</enddate><creator>Feldman, Anatol G</creator><general>Berlin/Heidelberg : Springer-Verlag</general><general>Springer-Verlag</general><general>Springer</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>FBQ</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>0-V</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QP</scope><scope>7QR</scope><scope>7RV</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7TM</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>88J</scope><scope>8AO</scope><scope>8FD</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>ALSLI</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FR3</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>KB0</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2R</scope><scope>NAPCQ</scope><scope>P64</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PRINS</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>RC3</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>20090301</creationdate><title>New insights into action-perception coupling</title><author>Feldman, Anatol G</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c454t-480efb5d3832ebd70fcd2c1d9fa890e19b0da2cb55d5d2d4405d5e408a40dac83</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2009</creationdate><topic>Algorithms</topic><topic>Ambiguity</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedicine</topic><topic>Brain - physiology</topic><topic>Brain research</topic><topic>Child development</topic><topic>Developmental psychology</topic><topic>Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision</topic><topic>Eye Movements</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Illusions</topic><topic>Models, Neurological</topic><topic>Motor Activity</topic><topic>Motor Neurons - physiology</topic><topic>Muscle, Skeletal - physiology</topic><topic>Neurology</topic><topic>Neurosciences</topic><topic>Newborn. Infant</topic><topic>Perception</topic><topic>Perceptions</topic><topic>Phantom Limb</topic><topic>Postural Balance</topic><topic>Posture</topic><topic>Proprioception</topic><topic>Proprioception - physiology</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Research Article</topic><topic>Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Feldman, Anatol G</creatorcontrib><collection>AGRIS</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Social Sciences Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts</collection><collection>Chemoreception Abstracts</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Nucleic Acids Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Social Science Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Pharma Collection</collection><collection>Technology Research Database</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>Social Science Premium Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Engineering Research Database</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Social Science Database</collection><collection>Nursing & Allied Health Premium</collection><collection>Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest Central China</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>Genetics Abstracts</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Feldman, Anatol G</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>New insights into action-perception coupling</atitle><jtitle>Experimental brain research</jtitle><stitle>Exp Brain Res</stitle><addtitle>Exp Brain Res</addtitle><date>2009-03-01</date><risdate>2009</risdate><volume>194</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>39</spage><epage>58</epage><pages>39-58</pages><issn>0014-4819</issn><eissn>1432-1106</eissn><coden>EXBRAP</coden><abstract>According to a view that has dominated the field for over a century, the brain programs muscle commands and uses a copy of these commands [efference copy (EC)] to adjust not only resulting motor action but also ongoing perception. This view was helpful in formulating several classical problems of action and perception: (1) the posture-movement problem of how movements away from a stable posture can be made without evoking resistance of posture-stabilizing mechanisms resulting from intrinsic muscle and reflex properties; (2) the problem of kinesthesia or why our sense of limb position is good despite ambiguous positional information delivered by proprioceptive and cutaneous signals; (3) the problem of visual space constancy or why the world is perceived as stable while its retinal image shifts following changes in gaze. On closer inspection, the EC theory actually does not solve these problems in a physiologically feasible way. Here solutions to these problems are proposed based on the advanced formulation of the equilibrium-point hypothesis that suggests that action and perception are accomplished in a common spatial frame of reference selected by the brain from a set of available frames. Experimental data suggest that the brain is also able to translate or/and rotate the selected frame of reference by modifying its major attributes--the origin, metrics and orientation--and thus substantially influence action and perception. Because of this ability, such frames are called physical to distinguish them from symbolic or mathematical frames that are used to describe system behavior without influencing this behavior. Experimental data also imply that once a frame of reference is chosen, its attributes are modified in a feedforward way, thus enabling the brain to act in an anticipatory and predictive manner. This approach is extended to sense of effort, kinesthetic illusions, phantom limb and phantom body phenomena. It also addresses the question of why retinal images of objects are sensed as objects located in the external, physical world, rather than in internal representations of the brain.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Berlin/Heidelberg : Springer-Verlag</pub><pmid>19082821</pmid><doi>10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3</doi><tpages>20</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0014-4819 |
ispartof | Experimental brain research, 2009-03, Vol.194 (1), p.39-58 |
issn | 0014-4819 1432-1106 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_67012504 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerLink Journals - AutoHoldings |
subjects | Algorithms Ambiguity Animals Biological and medical sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences Biomedicine Brain - physiology Brain research Child development Developmental psychology Eye and associated structures. Visual pathways and centers. Vision Eye Movements Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Humans Illusions Models, Neurological Motor Activity Motor Neurons - physiology Muscle, Skeletal - physiology Neurology Neurosciences Newborn. Infant Perception Perceptions Phantom Limb Postural Balance Posture Proprioception Proprioception - physiology Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Research Article Vertebrates: nervous system and sense organs |
title | New insights into action-perception coupling |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-08T17%3A32%3A51IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=New%20insights%20into%20action-perception%20coupling&rft.jtitle=Experimental%20brain%20research&rft.au=Feldman,%20Anatol%20G&rft.date=2009-03-01&rft.volume=194&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=39&rft.epage=58&rft.pages=39-58&rft.issn=0014-4819&rft.eissn=1432-1106&rft.coden=EXBRAP&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1658884581%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=215134833&rft_id=info:pmid/19082821&rfr_iscdi=true |