When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: implications for risk communication
Participants assessed the riskiness of 11 well‐known causes of death. Each participant was presented with an estimation of the number of deaths in the population due to that particular cause. The estimates were obtained from a previous study of naive participants' intuitive estimations. For ins...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Applied cognitive psychology 1997-12, Vol.11 (6), p.495-506 |
---|---|
1. Verfasser: | |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 506 |
---|---|
container_issue | 6 |
container_start_page | 495 |
container_title | Applied cognitive psychology |
container_volume | 11 |
creator | Yamagishi, Kimihiko |
description | Participants assessed the riskiness of 11 well‐known causes of death. Each participant was presented with an estimation of the number of deaths in the population due to that particular cause. The estimates were obtained from a previous study of naive participants' intuitive estimations. For instance, based on the result of the previous study, the number of deaths due to cancer was presented as: ‘2,414 out of 10,000’, ‘1,286 out of 10,000’, ‘24.14 out of 100’ and ‘12.86 out of 100’. The estimates of deaths were presented in analogous ways for the remaining ten causes of death. It was hypothesized that the judged degree of riskiness is affected by the number of deaths, irrespective of the total possible number (such as 10,000 or 100). Results from Experiment 1 were consistent with this prediction. Participants rated cancer as riskier when it was described as ‘kills 1,286 out of 10,000 people’ than as ‘kills 24.14 out of 100 people’, and similar results were observed regarding the remaining 10 causes of death. Experiment 2 replicated this trend. Implications for risk communications are discussed. © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199712)11:6<495::AID-ACP481>3.0.CO;2-J |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_57707359</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>1877054353</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c5051-61fd75f49a971c6aa3b14edd68849e0e5b69b21fac518bc81e1587c902ea1d233</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNqFkV9v0zAUxSMEEmXwHSzB0PaQ4OvEiV3QpCobpdVEkQoM8XLlOg7zlj_FTsX67UmUqg-AxNPVvTr-net7guACaASUsjdn60W-OAcqZUgzRs9AygzYOcA0fZdIPp3OFpfhLP-UCLiIIxrlq7csXD4KJscnj4MJFUKECRX0afDM-ztKqUyBTYLvN7emIYoAi0R6SurWdaqy3Z5YPzSGFKr5YVy786S7VQ1hSQTJ6ZTYeltZrTrbNp6UrSPO-nui27reNYf58-BJqSpvXhzqSfDl_dXn_EN4vZov8tl1qDnlEKZQFhkvE6n6b-lUqXgDiSmKVIhEGmr4JpUbBqXSHMRGCzDARaYlZUZBweL4JHg9creu_bkzvsPaem2qSjWm3xt5ltEs5rIXvvxDeNfuXNPvhiB6EU9iPuC-jirtWu-dKXHrbK3cHoHiEAjiEAgO18XhujgGggCYYh8IYh8IjoFgjBTzFTJc9uBXB3vltapKpxpt_ZHOaCJlLHrZt1H2y1Zm_5f5f7z_aX2Y9OhwRFvfmYcjWrl7TLM443jzcY6X8-WarucSRfwbEym3Og</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>1877054353</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: implications for risk communication</title><source>Wiley Journals</source><source>Periodicals Index Online</source><source>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</source><creator>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</creator><creatorcontrib>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</creatorcontrib><description>Participants assessed the riskiness of 11 well‐known causes of death. Each participant was presented with an estimation of the number of deaths in the population due to that particular cause. The estimates were obtained from a previous study of naive participants' intuitive estimations. For instance, based on the result of the previous study, the number of deaths due to cancer was presented as: ‘2,414 out of 10,000’, ‘1,286 out of 10,000’, ‘24.14 out of 100’ and ‘12.86 out of 100’. The estimates of deaths were presented in analogous ways for the remaining ten causes of death. It was hypothesized that the judged degree of riskiness is affected by the number of deaths, irrespective of the total possible number (such as 10,000 or 100). Results from Experiment 1 were consistent with this prediction. Participants rated cancer as riskier when it was described as ‘kills 1,286 out of 10,000 people’ than as ‘kills 24.14 out of 100 people’, and similar results were observed regarding the remaining 10 causes of death. Experiment 2 replicated this trend. Implications for risk communications are discussed. © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0888-4080</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1099-0720</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199712)11:6<495::AID-ACP481>3.0.CO;2-J</identifier><identifier>CODEN: ACPSED</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd</publisher><subject>Applied psychology ; Biological and medical sciences ; Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology ; Miscellaneous ; Mortality ; Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry ; Psychology. Psychophysiology ; Risk communication</subject><ispartof>Applied cognitive psychology, 1997-12, Vol.11 (6), p.495-506</ispartof><rights>Copyright © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.</rights><rights>1998 INIST-CNRS</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002%2F%28SICI%291099-0720%28199712%2911%3A6%3C495%3A%3AAID-ACP481%3E3.0.CO%3B2-J$$EPDF$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1002%2F%28SICI%291099-0720%28199712%2911%3A6%3C495%3A%3AAID-ACP481%3E3.0.CO%3B2-J$$EHTML$$P50$$Gwiley$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,1417,27869,27924,27925,31000,45574,45575</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://pascal-francis.inist.fr/vibad/index.php?action=getRecordDetail&idt=2049938$$DView record in Pascal Francis$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</creatorcontrib><title>When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: implications for risk communication</title><title>Applied cognitive psychology</title><addtitle>Appl. Cognit. Psychol</addtitle><description>Participants assessed the riskiness of 11 well‐known causes of death. Each participant was presented with an estimation of the number of deaths in the population due to that particular cause. The estimates were obtained from a previous study of naive participants' intuitive estimations. For instance, based on the result of the previous study, the number of deaths due to cancer was presented as: ‘2,414 out of 10,000’, ‘1,286 out of 10,000’, ‘24.14 out of 100’ and ‘12.86 out of 100’. The estimates of deaths were presented in analogous ways for the remaining ten causes of death. It was hypothesized that the judged degree of riskiness is affected by the number of deaths, irrespective of the total possible number (such as 10,000 or 100). Results from Experiment 1 were consistent with this prediction. Participants rated cancer as riskier when it was described as ‘kills 1,286 out of 10,000 people’ than as ‘kills 24.14 out of 100 people’, and similar results were observed regarding the remaining 10 causes of death. Experiment 2 replicated this trend. Implications for risk communications are discussed. © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.</description><subject>Applied psychology</subject><subject>Biological and medical sciences</subject><subject>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</subject><subject>Miscellaneous</subject><subject>Mortality</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</subject><subject>Psychology. Psychophysiology</subject><subject>Risk communication</subject><issn>0888-4080</issn><issn>1099-0720</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>1997</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>K30</sourceid><sourceid>7QJ</sourceid><recordid>eNqFkV9v0zAUxSMEEmXwHSzB0PaQ4OvEiV3QpCobpdVEkQoM8XLlOg7zlj_FTsX67UmUqg-AxNPVvTr-net7guACaASUsjdn60W-OAcqZUgzRs9AygzYOcA0fZdIPp3OFpfhLP-UCLiIIxrlq7csXD4KJscnj4MJFUKECRX0afDM-ztKqUyBTYLvN7emIYoAi0R6SurWdaqy3Z5YPzSGFKr5YVy786S7VQ1hSQTJ6ZTYeltZrTrbNp6UrSPO-nui27reNYf58-BJqSpvXhzqSfDl_dXn_EN4vZov8tl1qDnlEKZQFhkvE6n6b-lUqXgDiSmKVIhEGmr4JpUbBqXSHMRGCzDARaYlZUZBweL4JHg9creu_bkzvsPaem2qSjWm3xt5ltEs5rIXvvxDeNfuXNPvhiB6EU9iPuC-jirtWu-dKXHrbK3cHoHiEAjiEAgO18XhujgGggCYYh8IYh8IjoFgjBTzFTJc9uBXB3vltapKpxpt_ZHOaCJlLHrZt1H2y1Zm_5f5f7z_aX2Y9OhwRFvfmYcjWrl7TLM443jzcY6X8-WarucSRfwbEym3Og</recordid><startdate>199712</startdate><enddate>199712</enddate><creator>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</creator><general>John Wiley & Sons, Ltd</general><general>Wiley</general><scope>BSCLL</scope><scope>IQODW</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>JQCIK</scope><scope>K30</scope><scope>PAAUG</scope><scope>PAWHS</scope><scope>PAWZZ</scope><scope>PAXOH</scope><scope>PBHAV</scope><scope>PBQSW</scope><scope>PBYQZ</scope><scope>PCIWU</scope><scope>PCMID</scope><scope>PCZJX</scope><scope>PDGRG</scope><scope>PDWWI</scope><scope>PETMR</scope><scope>PFVGT</scope><scope>PGXDX</scope><scope>PIHIL</scope><scope>PISVA</scope><scope>PJCTQ</scope><scope>PJTMS</scope><scope>PLCHJ</scope><scope>PMHAD</scope><scope>PNQDJ</scope><scope>POUND</scope><scope>PPLAD</scope><scope>PQAPC</scope><scope>PQCAN</scope><scope>PQCMW</scope><scope>PQEME</scope><scope>PQHKH</scope><scope>PQMID</scope><scope>PQNCT</scope><scope>PQNET</scope><scope>PQSCT</scope><scope>PQSET</scope><scope>PSVJG</scope><scope>PVMQY</scope><scope>PZGFC</scope><scope>7QJ</scope></search><sort><creationdate>199712</creationdate><title>When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: implications for risk communication</title><author>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c5051-61fd75f49a971c6aa3b14edd68849e0e5b69b21fac518bc81e1587c902ea1d233</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>1997</creationdate><topic>Applied psychology</topic><topic>Biological and medical sciences</topic><topic>Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology</topic><topic>Miscellaneous</topic><topic>Mortality</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry</topic><topic>Psychology. Psychophysiology</topic><topic>Risk communication</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</creatorcontrib><collection>Istex</collection><collection>Pascal-Francis</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segment 33</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - International</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - West</collection><collection>Periodicals Index Online Segments 1-50</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - MEA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Canada</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - West</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - EMEALA</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Midwest</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - North Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Northeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - South Central</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access & Build (Plan A) - Southeast</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access (Plan D) - UK / I</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - APAC</collection><collection>Primary Sources Access—Foundation Edition (Plan E) - MEA</collection><collection>Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA)</collection><jtitle>Applied cognitive psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Yamagishi, Kimihiko</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: implications for risk communication</atitle><jtitle>Applied cognitive psychology</jtitle><addtitle>Appl. Cognit. Psychol</addtitle><date>1997-12</date><risdate>1997</risdate><volume>11</volume><issue>6</issue><spage>495</spage><epage>506</epage><pages>495-506</pages><issn>0888-4080</issn><eissn>1099-0720</eissn><coden>ACPSED</coden><abstract>Participants assessed the riskiness of 11 well‐known causes of death. Each participant was presented with an estimation of the number of deaths in the population due to that particular cause. The estimates were obtained from a previous study of naive participants' intuitive estimations. For instance, based on the result of the previous study, the number of deaths due to cancer was presented as: ‘2,414 out of 10,000’, ‘1,286 out of 10,000’, ‘24.14 out of 100’ and ‘12.86 out of 100’. The estimates of deaths were presented in analogous ways for the remaining ten causes of death. It was hypothesized that the judged degree of riskiness is affected by the number of deaths, irrespective of the total possible number (such as 10,000 or 100). Results from Experiment 1 were consistent with this prediction. Participants rated cancer as riskier when it was described as ‘kills 1,286 out of 10,000 people’ than as ‘kills 24.14 out of 100 people’, and similar results were observed regarding the remaining 10 causes of death. Experiment 2 replicated this trend. Implications for risk communications are discussed. © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.</abstract><cop>West Sussex</cop><pub>John Wiley & Sons, Ltd</pub><doi>10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199712)11:6<495::AID-ACP481>3.0.CO;2-J</doi><tpages>12</tpages></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0888-4080 |
ispartof | Applied cognitive psychology, 1997-12, Vol.11 (6), p.495-506 |
issn | 0888-4080 1099-0720 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_57707359 |
source | Wiley Journals; Periodicals Index Online; Applied Social Sciences Index & Abstracts (ASSIA) |
subjects | Applied psychology Biological and medical sciences Fundamental and applied biological sciences. Psychology Miscellaneous Mortality Psychology. Psychoanalysis. Psychiatry Psychology. Psychophysiology Risk communication |
title | When a 12.86% mortality is more dangerous than 24.14%: implications for risk communication |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-25T03%3A14%3A01IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=When%20a%2012.86%25%20mortality%20is%20more%20dangerous%20than%2024.14%25:%20implications%20for%20risk%20communication&rft.jtitle=Applied%20cognitive%20psychology&rft.au=Yamagishi,%20Kimihiko&rft.date=1997-12&rft.volume=11&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=495&rft.epage=506&rft.pages=495-506&rft.issn=0888-4080&rft.eissn=1099-0720&rft.coden=ACPSED&rft_id=info:doi/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199712)11:6%3C495::AID-ACP481%3E3.0.CO;2-J&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E1877054353%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=1877054353&rft_id=info:pmid/&rfr_iscdi=true |