Perception of saturation in natural objects
The distribution of colors across a surface depends on the interaction between its surface properties, its shape, and the lighting environment. Shading, chroma, and lightness are positively correlated: points on the object that have high luminance also have high chroma. Saturation, typically defined...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision Optics, image science, and vision, 2023-03, Vol.40 (3), p.A190-A198 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | A198 |
---|---|
container_issue | 3 |
container_start_page | A190 |
container_title | Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision |
container_volume | 40 |
creator | Hedjar, Laysa Toscani, Matteo Gegenfurtner, Karl R |
description | The distribution of colors across a surface depends on the interaction between its surface properties, its shape, and the lighting environment. Shading, chroma, and lightness are positively correlated: points on the object that have high luminance also have high chroma. Saturation, typically defined as the ratio of chroma to lightness, is therefore relatively constant across an object. Here we explored to what extent this relationship affects perceived saturation of an object. Using images of hyperspectral fruit and rendered matte objects, we manipulated the lightness-chroma correlation (positive or negative) and asked observers which of two objects appeared more saturated. Despite the negative-correlation stimulus having greater mean and maximum chroma, lightness, and saturation than the positive, observers overwhelmingly chose the positive as more saturated. This suggests that simple colorimetric statistics do not accurately represent perceived saturation of objects-observers likely base their judgments on interpretations about the cause of the color distribution. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1364/JOSAA.476874 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2809003704</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2809003704</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c329t-42c2e06562df312a225c1cb393a9906ca6486dfabef510cf600b467a5d88e91b3</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNo9kEtLw0AUhQdRbK3uXEuWgqbeeWZmWYpPChXU9TCZzEBKHnUmWfjvG5Pq6p4DH4fLh9A1hiWmgj28bT9WqyXLhMzYCZpjTiCVnJLTIYNkacaJmqGLGHcAwAbqHM1ohikFms3R3bsL1u27sm2S1ifRdH0wYyubpBlblbT5ztkuXqIzb6roro53gb6eHj_XL-lm-_y6Xm1SS4nqUkYscSC4IIWnmBhCuMU2p4oapUBYI5gUhTe58xyD9QIgZyIzvJDSKZzTBbqddveh_e5d7HRdRuuqyjSu7aMmEhQM3wMb0PsJtaGNMTiv96GsTfjRGPSvHj3q0ZOeAb85Lvd57Yp_-M8HPQCvAV60</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2809003704</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Perception of saturation in natural objects</title><source>Optica Publishing Group Journals</source><creator>Hedjar, Laysa ; Toscani, Matteo ; Gegenfurtner, Karl R</creator><creatorcontrib>Hedjar, Laysa ; Toscani, Matteo ; Gegenfurtner, Karl R</creatorcontrib><description>The distribution of colors across a surface depends on the interaction between its surface properties, its shape, and the lighting environment. Shading, chroma, and lightness are positively correlated: points on the object that have high luminance also have high chroma. Saturation, typically defined as the ratio of chroma to lightness, is therefore relatively constant across an object. Here we explored to what extent this relationship affects perceived saturation of an object. Using images of hyperspectral fruit and rendered matte objects, we manipulated the lightness-chroma correlation (positive or negative) and asked observers which of two objects appeared more saturated. Despite the negative-correlation stimulus having greater mean and maximum chroma, lightness, and saturation than the positive, observers overwhelmingly chose the positive as more saturated. This suggests that simple colorimetric statistics do not accurately represent perceived saturation of objects-observers likely base their judgments on interpretations about the cause of the color distribution.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1084-7529</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1520-8532</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1364/JOSAA.476874</identifier><identifier>PMID: 37133037</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States</publisher><ispartof>Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision, 2023-03, Vol.40 (3), p.A190-A198</ispartof><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c329t-42c2e06562df312a225c1cb393a9906ca6486dfabef510cf600b467a5d88e91b3</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c329t-42c2e06562df312a225c1cb393a9906ca6486dfabef510cf600b467a5d88e91b3</cites><orcidid>0000-0001-5390-0684</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3258,27924,27925</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/37133037$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Hedjar, Laysa</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Toscani, Matteo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gegenfurtner, Karl R</creatorcontrib><title>Perception of saturation in natural objects</title><title>Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision</title><addtitle>J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis</addtitle><description>The distribution of colors across a surface depends on the interaction between its surface properties, its shape, and the lighting environment. Shading, chroma, and lightness are positively correlated: points on the object that have high luminance also have high chroma. Saturation, typically defined as the ratio of chroma to lightness, is therefore relatively constant across an object. Here we explored to what extent this relationship affects perceived saturation of an object. Using images of hyperspectral fruit and rendered matte objects, we manipulated the lightness-chroma correlation (positive or negative) and asked observers which of two objects appeared more saturated. Despite the negative-correlation stimulus having greater mean and maximum chroma, lightness, and saturation than the positive, observers overwhelmingly chose the positive as more saturated. This suggests that simple colorimetric statistics do not accurately represent perceived saturation of objects-observers likely base their judgments on interpretations about the cause of the color distribution.</description><issn>1084-7529</issn><issn>1520-8532</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNo9kEtLw0AUhQdRbK3uXEuWgqbeeWZmWYpPChXU9TCZzEBKHnUmWfjvG5Pq6p4DH4fLh9A1hiWmgj28bT9WqyXLhMzYCZpjTiCVnJLTIYNkacaJmqGLGHcAwAbqHM1ohikFms3R3bsL1u27sm2S1ifRdH0wYyubpBlblbT5ztkuXqIzb6roro53gb6eHj_XL-lm-_y6Xm1SS4nqUkYscSC4IIWnmBhCuMU2p4oapUBYI5gUhTe58xyD9QIgZyIzvJDSKZzTBbqddveh_e5d7HRdRuuqyjSu7aMmEhQM3wMb0PsJtaGNMTiv96GsTfjRGPSvHj3q0ZOeAb85Lvd57Yp_-M8HPQCvAV60</recordid><startdate>20230301</startdate><enddate>20230301</enddate><creator>Hedjar, Laysa</creator><creator>Toscani, Matteo</creator><creator>Gegenfurtner, Karl R</creator><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5390-0684</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20230301</creationdate><title>Perception of saturation in natural objects</title><author>Hedjar, Laysa ; Toscani, Matteo ; Gegenfurtner, Karl R</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c329t-42c2e06562df312a225c1cb393a9906ca6486dfabef510cf600b467a5d88e91b3</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Hedjar, Laysa</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Toscani, Matteo</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Gegenfurtner, Karl R</creatorcontrib><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Hedjar, Laysa</au><au>Toscani, Matteo</au><au>Gegenfurtner, Karl R</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Perception of saturation in natural objects</atitle><jtitle>Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision</jtitle><addtitle>J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis</addtitle><date>2023-03-01</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>40</volume><issue>3</issue><spage>A190</spage><epage>A198</epage><pages>A190-A198</pages><issn>1084-7529</issn><eissn>1520-8532</eissn><abstract>The distribution of colors across a surface depends on the interaction between its surface properties, its shape, and the lighting environment. Shading, chroma, and lightness are positively correlated: points on the object that have high luminance also have high chroma. Saturation, typically defined as the ratio of chroma to lightness, is therefore relatively constant across an object. Here we explored to what extent this relationship affects perceived saturation of an object. Using images of hyperspectral fruit and rendered matte objects, we manipulated the lightness-chroma correlation (positive or negative) and asked observers which of two objects appeared more saturated. Despite the negative-correlation stimulus having greater mean and maximum chroma, lightness, and saturation than the positive, observers overwhelmingly chose the positive as more saturated. This suggests that simple colorimetric statistics do not accurately represent perceived saturation of objects-observers likely base their judgments on interpretations about the cause of the color distribution.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pmid>37133037</pmid><doi>10.1364/JOSAA.476874</doi><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5390-0684</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1084-7529 |
ispartof | Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision, 2023-03, Vol.40 (3), p.A190-A198 |
issn | 1084-7529 1520-8532 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2809003704 |
source | Optica Publishing Group Journals |
title | Perception of saturation in natural objects |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-24T21%3A44%3A01IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Perception%20of%20saturation%20in%20natural%20objects&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20the%20Optical%20Society%20of%20America.%20A,%20Optics,%20image%20science,%20and%20vision&rft.au=Hedjar,%20Laysa&rft.date=2023-03-01&rft.volume=40&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=A190&rft.epage=A198&rft.pages=A190-A198&rft.issn=1084-7529&rft.eissn=1520-8532&rft_id=info:doi/10.1364/JOSAA.476874&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2809003704%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2809003704&rft_id=info:pmid/37133037&rfr_iscdi=true |