Characteristics of Early Expressive Vocabulary in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants
Purpose: The aim of this study is to evaluate whether Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants (CIs) demonstrated early lexical composition similar to their hearing peers who were at the same vocabulary level and the extent to which children with CIs were sensitive to linguistic and concept...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of speech, language, and hearing research language, and hearing research, 2022-11, Vol.65 (11), p.4369-4384 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , , , , , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 4384 |
---|---|
container_issue | 11 |
container_start_page | 4369 |
container_title | Journal of speech, language, and hearing research |
container_volume | 65 |
creator | Luo, Jianfen Xu, Lei Wang, Min Xie, Dianzhao Li, Jinming Liu, Xianqi He, Shuman Spencer, Linda Rost, Gwyneth Guo, Ling-Yu |
description | Purpose: The aim of this study is to evaluate whether Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants (CIs) demonstrated early lexical composition similar to their hearing peers who were at the same vocabulary level and the extent to which children with CIs were sensitive to linguistic and conceptual properties when developing early lexicon. Method: Participants were 77 Mandarin-speaking children with CIs who received CIs before 30 months of age. Their expressive vocabulary was documented using the Infant Checklist of the Early Vocabulary Inventory for Mandarin Chinese 9 or 12 months after CI activation. Percent social words, common nouns, predicates (verbs, adjectives), and closed-class words in total vocabulary were computed for children at different vocabulary levels. Common nouns and verbs were further coded for their word class (noun, verb), word frequency, word length, and imageability to predict how likely a given noun or verb would be produced by children with CIs. Results: Like children with typical hearing, social words were the most dominant category when vocabulary size in children with CIs was smaller than 20 words; common nouns became the most dominant category when the vocabulary size reached 21 words. The difference in percent common nouns and percent predicates (i.e., noun bias) was similar in children with CIs and their hearing peers. In addition, verbs, common words, monosyllabic words, and more imageable words were more likely to be produced by children with CIs than their counterparts. Conclusions: Mandarin children with CIs showed language-specific patterns in early lexical composition like their hearing peers. They were able to use multiple linguistic and conceptual cues when approaching early expressive vocabulary despite perceptual and processing constraints. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00183 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>gale_proqu</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2728464593</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><galeid>A728377835</galeid><ericid>EJ1368561</ericid><sourcerecordid>A728377835</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c463t-4a37b4e2262974e7e408d18295fc6be5c61caab0074550c8736227d27bce6d483</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNptkl9v1SAYxhujcXP6CYymiYnZTSf_CpzLpTm6LceYOPWWUPr2FKXQQavu24955nTJgQvewO8h8D5PUbzE6AQjxt4RRIi6uNycfa4IqRDCkj4qDnFdy2qFEXmca7QiFaNSHhTPUvqO8sCMPy0OKCeScMkOC9MMOmozQ7RptiaVoS_XOrrrcv17ipCS_Qnlt2B0uzgdr0vry4_adzpaX11OoH9Yvy2bwbougi9_2Xkom2AGBzqW5-PktJ_T8-JJr12CF3frUfH1_fpLc1ZtPn04b043lWGczhXTVLQMCOFkJRgIYEh2WJJV3RveQm04Nlq3CAlW18hIkX9BREdEa4B3TNKj4nh37xTD1QJpVqNNBlx-BIQlKSKIZJzVK5rRNzt0qx0o6_sw5y7c4uo0U1QISetMVXuoLXiI2gUPvc3bD_iTPXyeHYzW7BW8_U8wgHbzkIJbZht8egjSHWhiSClCr6Zox2yIwkjdpkH9S4PKxZ80ZNXru4Ys7Qjdveav_Rl4tQOy_eb-eH2BKZc1x_QGW1i3aw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2728464593</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Characteristics of Early Expressive Vocabulary in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>Education Source</source><source>Alma/SFX Local Collection</source><creator>Luo, Jianfen ; Xu, Lei ; Wang, Min ; Xie, Dianzhao ; Li, Jinming ; Liu, Xianqi ; He, Shuman ; Spencer, Linda ; Rost, Gwyneth ; Guo, Ling-Yu</creator><creatorcontrib>Luo, Jianfen ; Xu, Lei ; Wang, Min ; Xie, Dianzhao ; Li, Jinming ; Liu, Xianqi ; He, Shuman ; Spencer, Linda ; Rost, Gwyneth ; Guo, Ling-Yu</creatorcontrib><description>Purpose: The aim of this study is to evaluate whether Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants (CIs) demonstrated early lexical composition similar to their hearing peers who were at the same vocabulary level and the extent to which children with CIs were sensitive to linguistic and conceptual properties when developing early lexicon. Method: Participants were 77 Mandarin-speaking children with CIs who received CIs before 30 months of age. Their expressive vocabulary was documented using the Infant Checklist of the Early Vocabulary Inventory for Mandarin Chinese 9 or 12 months after CI activation. Percent social words, common nouns, predicates (verbs, adjectives), and closed-class words in total vocabulary were computed for children at different vocabulary levels. Common nouns and verbs were further coded for their word class (noun, verb), word frequency, word length, and imageability to predict how likely a given noun or verb would be produced by children with CIs. Results: Like children with typical hearing, social words were the most dominant category when vocabulary size in children with CIs was smaller than 20 words; common nouns became the most dominant category when the vocabulary size reached 21 words. The difference in percent common nouns and percent predicates (i.e., noun bias) was similar in children with CIs and their hearing peers. In addition, verbs, common words, monosyllabic words, and more imageable words were more likely to be produced by children with CIs than their counterparts. Conclusions: Mandarin children with CIs showed language-specific patterns in early lexical composition like their hearing peers. They were able to use multiple linguistic and conceptual cues when approaching early expressive vocabulary despite perceptual and processing constraints.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1092-4388</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1558-9102</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00183</identifier><identifier>PMID: 36282684</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association</publisher><subject>Analysis ; Assistive Technology ; Care and treatment ; Child ; Cochlear Implantation ; Cochlear Implants ; Cues ; Diagnosis ; Expressive Language ; Foreign Countries ; Form Classes (Languages) ; Health aspects ; Hearing disorders in children ; Hearing Impairments ; Humans ; Infant ; Infants ; Language ; Language Development ; Language Patterns ; Mandarin Chinese ; Nouns ; Verbs ; Vocabulary ; Vocabulary Development ; Vocabulary tests</subject><ispartof>Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 2022-11, Vol.65 (11), p.4369-4384</ispartof><rights>COPYRIGHT 2022 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c463t-4a37b4e2262974e7e408d18295fc6be5c61caab0074550c8736227d27bce6d483</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c463t-4a37b4e2262974e7e408d18295fc6be5c61caab0074550c8736227d27bce6d483</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-9143-3383</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,27901,27902</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttp://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1368561$$DView record in ERIC$$Hfree_for_read</backlink><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36282684$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Luo, Jianfen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Xu, Lei</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wang, Min</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Xie, Dianzhao</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Li, Jinming</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liu, Xianqi</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>He, Shuman</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spencer, Linda</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rost, Gwyneth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Guo, Ling-Yu</creatorcontrib><title>Characteristics of Early Expressive Vocabulary in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants</title><title>Journal of speech, language, and hearing research</title><addtitle>J Speech Lang Hear Res</addtitle><description>Purpose: The aim of this study is to evaluate whether Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants (CIs) demonstrated early lexical composition similar to their hearing peers who were at the same vocabulary level and the extent to which children with CIs were sensitive to linguistic and conceptual properties when developing early lexicon. Method: Participants were 77 Mandarin-speaking children with CIs who received CIs before 30 months of age. Their expressive vocabulary was documented using the Infant Checklist of the Early Vocabulary Inventory for Mandarin Chinese 9 or 12 months after CI activation. Percent social words, common nouns, predicates (verbs, adjectives), and closed-class words in total vocabulary were computed for children at different vocabulary levels. Common nouns and verbs were further coded for their word class (noun, verb), word frequency, word length, and imageability to predict how likely a given noun or verb would be produced by children with CIs. Results: Like children with typical hearing, social words were the most dominant category when vocabulary size in children with CIs was smaller than 20 words; common nouns became the most dominant category when the vocabulary size reached 21 words. The difference in percent common nouns and percent predicates (i.e., noun bias) was similar in children with CIs and their hearing peers. In addition, verbs, common words, monosyllabic words, and more imageable words were more likely to be produced by children with CIs than their counterparts. Conclusions: Mandarin children with CIs showed language-specific patterns in early lexical composition like their hearing peers. They were able to use multiple linguistic and conceptual cues when approaching early expressive vocabulary despite perceptual and processing constraints.</description><subject>Analysis</subject><subject>Assistive Technology</subject><subject>Care and treatment</subject><subject>Child</subject><subject>Cochlear Implantation</subject><subject>Cochlear Implants</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Diagnosis</subject><subject>Expressive Language</subject><subject>Foreign Countries</subject><subject>Form Classes (Languages)</subject><subject>Health aspects</subject><subject>Hearing disorders in children</subject><subject>Hearing Impairments</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Infant</subject><subject>Infants</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Language Development</subject><subject>Language Patterns</subject><subject>Mandarin Chinese</subject><subject>Nouns</subject><subject>Verbs</subject><subject>Vocabulary</subject><subject>Vocabulary Development</subject><subject>Vocabulary tests</subject><issn>1092-4388</issn><issn>1558-9102</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNptkl9v1SAYxhujcXP6CYymiYnZTSf_CpzLpTm6LceYOPWWUPr2FKXQQavu24955nTJgQvewO8h8D5PUbzE6AQjxt4RRIi6uNycfa4IqRDCkj4qDnFdy2qFEXmca7QiFaNSHhTPUvqO8sCMPy0OKCeScMkOC9MMOmozQ7RptiaVoS_XOrrrcv17ipCS_Qnlt2B0uzgdr0vry4_adzpaX11OoH9Yvy2bwbougi9_2Xkom2AGBzqW5-PktJ_T8-JJr12CF3frUfH1_fpLc1ZtPn04b043lWGczhXTVLQMCOFkJRgIYEh2WJJV3RveQm04Nlq3CAlW18hIkX9BREdEa4B3TNKj4nh37xTD1QJpVqNNBlx-BIQlKSKIZJzVK5rRNzt0qx0o6_sw5y7c4uo0U1QISetMVXuoLXiI2gUPvc3bD_iTPXyeHYzW7BW8_U8wgHbzkIJbZht8egjSHWhiSClCr6Zox2yIwkjdpkH9S4PKxZ80ZNXru4Ys7Qjdveav_Rl4tQOy_eb-eH2BKZc1x_QGW1i3aw</recordid><startdate>20221101</startdate><enddate>20221101</enddate><creator>Luo, Jianfen</creator><creator>Xu, Lei</creator><creator>Wang, Min</creator><creator>Xie, Dianzhao</creator><creator>Li, Jinming</creator><creator>Liu, Xianqi</creator><creator>He, Shuman</creator><creator>Spencer, Linda</creator><creator>Rost, Gwyneth</creator><creator>Guo, Ling-Yu</creator><general>American Speech-Language-Hearing Association</general><scope>7SW</scope><scope>BJH</scope><scope>BNH</scope><scope>BNI</scope><scope>BNJ</scope><scope>BNO</scope><scope>ERI</scope><scope>PET</scope><scope>REK</scope><scope>WWN</scope><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9143-3383</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20221101</creationdate><title>Characteristics of Early Expressive Vocabulary in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants</title><author>Luo, Jianfen ; Xu, Lei ; Wang, Min ; Xie, Dianzhao ; Li, Jinming ; Liu, Xianqi ; He, Shuman ; Spencer, Linda ; Rost, Gwyneth ; Guo, Ling-Yu</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c463t-4a37b4e2262974e7e408d18295fc6be5c61caab0074550c8736227d27bce6d483</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Analysis</topic><topic>Assistive Technology</topic><topic>Care and treatment</topic><topic>Child</topic><topic>Cochlear Implantation</topic><topic>Cochlear Implants</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Diagnosis</topic><topic>Expressive Language</topic><topic>Foreign Countries</topic><topic>Form Classes (Languages)</topic><topic>Health aspects</topic><topic>Hearing disorders in children</topic><topic>Hearing Impairments</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Infant</topic><topic>Infants</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Language Development</topic><topic>Language Patterns</topic><topic>Mandarin Chinese</topic><topic>Nouns</topic><topic>Verbs</topic><topic>Vocabulary</topic><topic>Vocabulary Development</topic><topic>Vocabulary tests</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Luo, Jianfen</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Xu, Lei</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Wang, Min</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Xie, Dianzhao</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Li, Jinming</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Liu, Xianqi</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>He, Shuman</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Spencer, Linda</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Rost, Gwyneth</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Guo, Ling-Yu</creatorcontrib><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Ovid)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>ERIC( SilverPlatter )</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)</collection><collection>Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)</collection><collection>ERIC</collection><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Journal of speech, language, and hearing research</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Luo, Jianfen</au><au>Xu, Lei</au><au>Wang, Min</au><au>Xie, Dianzhao</au><au>Li, Jinming</au><au>Liu, Xianqi</au><au>He, Shuman</au><au>Spencer, Linda</au><au>Rost, Gwyneth</au><au>Guo, Ling-Yu</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><ericid>EJ1368561</ericid><atitle>Characteristics of Early Expressive Vocabulary in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants</atitle><jtitle>Journal of speech, language, and hearing research</jtitle><addtitle>J Speech Lang Hear Res</addtitle><date>2022-11-01</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>65</volume><issue>11</issue><spage>4369</spage><epage>4384</epage><pages>4369-4384</pages><issn>1092-4388</issn><eissn>1558-9102</eissn><abstract>Purpose: The aim of this study is to evaluate whether Mandarin-speaking children with cochlear implants (CIs) demonstrated early lexical composition similar to their hearing peers who were at the same vocabulary level and the extent to which children with CIs were sensitive to linguistic and conceptual properties when developing early lexicon. Method: Participants were 77 Mandarin-speaking children with CIs who received CIs before 30 months of age. Their expressive vocabulary was documented using the Infant Checklist of the Early Vocabulary Inventory for Mandarin Chinese 9 or 12 months after CI activation. Percent social words, common nouns, predicates (verbs, adjectives), and closed-class words in total vocabulary were computed for children at different vocabulary levels. Common nouns and verbs were further coded for their word class (noun, verb), word frequency, word length, and imageability to predict how likely a given noun or verb would be produced by children with CIs. Results: Like children with typical hearing, social words were the most dominant category when vocabulary size in children with CIs was smaller than 20 words; common nouns became the most dominant category when the vocabulary size reached 21 words. The difference in percent common nouns and percent predicates (i.e., noun bias) was similar in children with CIs and their hearing peers. In addition, verbs, common words, monosyllabic words, and more imageable words were more likely to be produced by children with CIs than their counterparts. Conclusions: Mandarin children with CIs showed language-specific patterns in early lexical composition like their hearing peers. They were able to use multiple linguistic and conceptual cues when approaching early expressive vocabulary despite perceptual and processing constraints.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>American Speech-Language-Hearing Association</pub><pmid>36282684</pmid><doi>10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00183</doi><tpages>16</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9143-3383</orcidid></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1092-4388 |
ispartof | Journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 2022-11, Vol.65 (11), p.4369-4384 |
issn | 1092-4388 1558-9102 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2728464593 |
source | MEDLINE; Education Source; Alma/SFX Local Collection |
subjects | Analysis Assistive Technology Care and treatment Child Cochlear Implantation Cochlear Implants Cues Diagnosis Expressive Language Foreign Countries Form Classes (Languages) Health aspects Hearing disorders in children Hearing Impairments Humans Infant Infants Language Language Development Language Patterns Mandarin Chinese Nouns Verbs Vocabulary Vocabulary Development Vocabulary tests |
title | Characteristics of Early Expressive Vocabulary in Mandarin-Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-31T07%3A49%3A00IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-gale_proqu&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Characteristics%20of%20Early%20Expressive%20Vocabulary%20in%20Mandarin-Speaking%20Children%20with%20Cochlear%20Implants&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20speech,%20language,%20and%20hearing%20research&rft.au=Luo,%20Jianfen&rft.date=2022-11-01&rft.volume=65&rft.issue=11&rft.spage=4369&rft.epage=4384&rft.pages=4369-4384&rft.issn=1092-4388&rft.eissn=1558-9102&rft_id=info:doi/10.1044/2022_JSLHR-22-00183&rft_dat=%3Cgale_proqu%3EA728377835%3C/gale_proqu%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2728464593&rft_id=info:pmid/36282684&rft_galeid=A728377835&rft_ericid=EJ1368561&rfr_iscdi=true |