Playing it by ear: gregarious sparrows recognize and respond to isolated wingbeat sounds and predator-based cues
The ability to detect an incoming attack provides a final opportunity for an animal to avoid predation. In birds, vision is the main sensory mode in detecting attacks, but auditory cues likely play an important role. The role of auditory cues from predators themselves remains largely unstudied. We e...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Animal cognition 2023-03, Vol.26 (2), p.357-367 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 367 |
---|---|
container_issue | 2 |
container_start_page | 357 |
container_title | Animal cognition |
container_volume | 26 |
creator | Shearer, David J. Beilke, Elizabeth A. |
description | The ability to detect an incoming attack provides a final opportunity for an animal to avoid predation. In birds, vision is the main sensory mode in detecting attacks, but auditory cues likely play an important role. The role of auditory cues from predators themselves remains largely unstudied. We evaluated the ability of free-living, gregarious sparrows (Passerellidae) to recognize attacks based on the non-vocal sounds made by predators or indirect auditory cues of ongoing attacks, mainly in the form of brief wingbeat sequences from predatory and non-predatory birds. Behavioral responses to playbacks were video-recorded and expressed in terms of a flock’s propensity to respond, either by flushing to cover, becoming vigilant, or both. Sparrows responded equally to hawk wingbeats and those of small passerines. Both predator and non-predator wingbeat sequences induced anti-predator responses, especially when played loudly. Loud control sounds, such as hammering, induced few responses. Birds also responded to the sounds of a walking and running terrestrial predator (a dog), but reactions to the walking predator often involved birds jumping onto objects for a better view of their surroundings rather than immediate flight to cover. In an additional experiment, we examined how characteristics of wingbeat sequences (i.e., the number and cadence of hawk wingbeats) affected passerine responses. It indicated that only two consecutive hawk wingbeats, presented at a natural cadence, are necessary to elicit a strong response to a playback. Single hawk wingbeats induced only weak escape responses, as did artificially slowed cadences. Birds in general likely possess the ability to recognize non-vocal, auditory cues of incoming attacks, which may be produced by approaching threats or departing congeners. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1007/s10071-022-01663-z |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2699704831</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sourcerecordid>2699704831</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-dec433eb1c13e2a603a46fc9756117e9919e5a84fabe439055b900e5b30555893</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kU1LxDAQhoMofqz-AQ8S8OKlmjRJ03iTxS8Q9KDnkLazpdJtaqZl2f31RnddwYOXzAx53ncSXkJOObvkjOkr_Dp5wtI0YTzLRLLaIYdcCpUYqbLdbS_zA3KE-M4Yy6Xh--RAKCOiRB2S_qV1y6araTPQYknBhWtaB6hdaPyIFHsXgl8gDVD6umtWQF1XxQl7H-vgaYO-dQNUdBFdCnADRT92FX5zfYDKDT4khcOIlCPgMdmbuRbhZFMn5O3u9nX6kDw93z9Ob56SUmg1JBWUUggoeMkFpC5jwslsVhqtMs41GMMNKJfLmStACsOUKgxjoAoRW5UbMSEXa98--I-4d7DzBktoW9dB_JlNM2M0k7ngET3_g777MXTxdTbV2qRKGy0jla6pMnjEADPbh2buwtJyZr-SsOs8bMzDfudhV1F0trEeizlUW8lPABEQawDjVVdD-N39j-0nGaWWtg</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2779257974</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Playing it by ear: gregarious sparrows recognize and respond to isolated wingbeat sounds and predator-based cues</title><source>MEDLINE</source><source>SpringerNature Journals</source><creator>Shearer, David J. ; Beilke, Elizabeth A.</creator><creatorcontrib>Shearer, David J. ; Beilke, Elizabeth A.</creatorcontrib><description>The ability to detect an incoming attack provides a final opportunity for an animal to avoid predation. In birds, vision is the main sensory mode in detecting attacks, but auditory cues likely play an important role. The role of auditory cues from predators themselves remains largely unstudied. We evaluated the ability of free-living, gregarious sparrows (Passerellidae) to recognize attacks based on the non-vocal sounds made by predators or indirect auditory cues of ongoing attacks, mainly in the form of brief wingbeat sequences from predatory and non-predatory birds. Behavioral responses to playbacks were video-recorded and expressed in terms of a flock’s propensity to respond, either by flushing to cover, becoming vigilant, or both. Sparrows responded equally to hawk wingbeats and those of small passerines. Both predator and non-predator wingbeat sequences induced anti-predator responses, especially when played loudly. Loud control sounds, such as hammering, induced few responses. Birds also responded to the sounds of a walking and running terrestrial predator (a dog), but reactions to the walking predator often involved birds jumping onto objects for a better view of their surroundings rather than immediate flight to cover. In an additional experiment, we examined how characteristics of wingbeat sequences (i.e., the number and cadence of hawk wingbeats) affected passerine responses. It indicated that only two consecutive hawk wingbeats, presented at a natural cadence, are necessary to elicit a strong response to a playback. Single hawk wingbeats induced only weak escape responses, as did artificially slowed cadences. Birds in general likely possess the ability to recognize non-vocal, auditory cues of incoming attacks, which may be produced by approaching threats or departing congeners.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1435-9448</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1435-9456</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1007/s10071-022-01663-z</identifier><identifier>PMID: 35930165</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg</publisher><subject>Animal cognition ; Animals ; Anti-predator behavior ; Behavior ; Behavioral Sciences ; Biomedical and Life Sciences ; Birds ; Birds of prey ; Congeners ; Cues ; Dogs ; Escape behavior ; Experiments ; Hearing ; Life Sciences ; Original Paper ; Playback ; Playbacks ; Predation ; Predators ; Predatory Behavior - physiology ; Psychology Research ; Sequences ; Sparrows ; Walking ; Zoology</subject><ispartof>Animal cognition, 2023-03, Vol.26 (2), p.357-367</ispartof><rights>The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2022. Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.</rights><rights>2022. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature.</rights><rights>The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2022. Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-dec433eb1c13e2a603a46fc9756117e9919e5a84fabe439055b900e5b30555893</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-dec433eb1c13e2a603a46fc9756117e9919e5a84fabe439055b900e5b30555893</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-7364-7521 ; 0000-0003-3941-8722</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-022-01663-z$$EPDF$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10071-022-01663-z$$EHTML$$P50$$Gspringer$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,27924,27925,41488,42557,51319</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35930165$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Shearer, David J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Beilke, Elizabeth A.</creatorcontrib><title>Playing it by ear: gregarious sparrows recognize and respond to isolated wingbeat sounds and predator-based cues</title><title>Animal cognition</title><addtitle>Anim Cogn</addtitle><addtitle>Anim Cogn</addtitle><description>The ability to detect an incoming attack provides a final opportunity for an animal to avoid predation. In birds, vision is the main sensory mode in detecting attacks, but auditory cues likely play an important role. The role of auditory cues from predators themselves remains largely unstudied. We evaluated the ability of free-living, gregarious sparrows (Passerellidae) to recognize attacks based on the non-vocal sounds made by predators or indirect auditory cues of ongoing attacks, mainly in the form of brief wingbeat sequences from predatory and non-predatory birds. Behavioral responses to playbacks were video-recorded and expressed in terms of a flock’s propensity to respond, either by flushing to cover, becoming vigilant, or both. Sparrows responded equally to hawk wingbeats and those of small passerines. Both predator and non-predator wingbeat sequences induced anti-predator responses, especially when played loudly. Loud control sounds, such as hammering, induced few responses. Birds also responded to the sounds of a walking and running terrestrial predator (a dog), but reactions to the walking predator often involved birds jumping onto objects for a better view of their surroundings rather than immediate flight to cover. In an additional experiment, we examined how characteristics of wingbeat sequences (i.e., the number and cadence of hawk wingbeats) affected passerine responses. It indicated that only two consecutive hawk wingbeats, presented at a natural cadence, are necessary to elicit a strong response to a playback. Single hawk wingbeats induced only weak escape responses, as did artificially slowed cadences. Birds in general likely possess the ability to recognize non-vocal, auditory cues of incoming attacks, which may be produced by approaching threats or departing congeners.</description><subject>Animal cognition</subject><subject>Animals</subject><subject>Anti-predator behavior</subject><subject>Behavior</subject><subject>Behavioral Sciences</subject><subject>Biomedical and Life Sciences</subject><subject>Birds</subject><subject>Birds of prey</subject><subject>Congeners</subject><subject>Cues</subject><subject>Dogs</subject><subject>Escape behavior</subject><subject>Experiments</subject><subject>Hearing</subject><subject>Life Sciences</subject><subject>Original Paper</subject><subject>Playback</subject><subject>Playbacks</subject><subject>Predation</subject><subject>Predators</subject><subject>Predatory Behavior - physiology</subject><subject>Psychology Research</subject><subject>Sequences</subject><subject>Sparrows</subject><subject>Walking</subject><subject>Zoology</subject><issn>1435-9448</issn><issn>1435-9456</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2023</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><sourceid>8G5</sourceid><sourceid>ABUWG</sourceid><sourceid>AFKRA</sourceid><sourceid>AZQEC</sourceid><sourceid>BENPR</sourceid><sourceid>CCPQU</sourceid><sourceid>DWQXO</sourceid><sourceid>GNUQQ</sourceid><sourceid>GUQSH</sourceid><sourceid>M2O</sourceid><recordid>eNp9kU1LxDAQhoMofqz-AQ8S8OKlmjRJ03iTxS8Q9KDnkLazpdJtaqZl2f31RnddwYOXzAx53ncSXkJOObvkjOkr_Dp5wtI0YTzLRLLaIYdcCpUYqbLdbS_zA3KE-M4Yy6Xh--RAKCOiRB2S_qV1y6araTPQYknBhWtaB6hdaPyIFHsXgl8gDVD6umtWQF1XxQl7H-vgaYO-dQNUdBFdCnADRT92FX5zfYDKDT4khcOIlCPgMdmbuRbhZFMn5O3u9nX6kDw93z9Ob56SUmg1JBWUUggoeMkFpC5jwslsVhqtMs41GMMNKJfLmStACsOUKgxjoAoRW5UbMSEXa98--I-4d7DzBktoW9dB_JlNM2M0k7ngET3_g777MXTxdTbV2qRKGy0jla6pMnjEADPbh2buwtJyZr-SsOs8bMzDfudhV1F0trEeizlUW8lPABEQawDjVVdD-N39j-0nGaWWtg</recordid><startdate>20230301</startdate><enddate>20230301</enddate><creator>Shearer, David J.</creator><creator>Beilke, Elizabeth A.</creator><general>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</general><general>Springer Nature B.V</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>3V.</scope><scope>7QG</scope><scope>7SN</scope><scope>7TK</scope><scope>7X7</scope><scope>7XB</scope><scope>88E</scope><scope>88G</scope><scope>8FI</scope><scope>8FJ</scope><scope>8FK</scope><scope>8G5</scope><scope>ABUWG</scope><scope>AFKRA</scope><scope>AZQEC</scope><scope>BENPR</scope><scope>C1K</scope><scope>CCPQU</scope><scope>DWQXO</scope><scope>FYUFA</scope><scope>GHDGH</scope><scope>GNUQQ</scope><scope>GUQSH</scope><scope>K9.</scope><scope>M0S</scope><scope>M1P</scope><scope>M2M</scope><scope>M2O</scope><scope>MBDVC</scope><scope>PQEST</scope><scope>PQQKQ</scope><scope>PQUKI</scope><scope>PSYQQ</scope><scope>Q9U</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7364-7521</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3941-8722</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>20230301</creationdate><title>Playing it by ear: gregarious sparrows recognize and respond to isolated wingbeat sounds and predator-based cues</title><author>Shearer, David J. ; Beilke, Elizabeth A.</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c375t-dec433eb1c13e2a603a46fc9756117e9919e5a84fabe439055b900e5b30555893</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2023</creationdate><topic>Animal cognition</topic><topic>Animals</topic><topic>Anti-predator behavior</topic><topic>Behavior</topic><topic>Behavioral Sciences</topic><topic>Biomedical and Life Sciences</topic><topic>Birds</topic><topic>Birds of prey</topic><topic>Congeners</topic><topic>Cues</topic><topic>Dogs</topic><topic>Escape behavior</topic><topic>Experiments</topic><topic>Hearing</topic><topic>Life Sciences</topic><topic>Original Paper</topic><topic>Playback</topic><topic>Playbacks</topic><topic>Predation</topic><topic>Predators</topic><topic>Predatory Behavior - physiology</topic><topic>Psychology Research</topic><topic>Sequences</topic><topic>Sparrows</topic><topic>Walking</topic><topic>Zoology</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Shearer, David J.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Beilke, Elizabeth A.</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Corporate)</collection><collection>Animal Behavior Abstracts</collection><collection>Ecology Abstracts</collection><collection>Neurosciences Abstracts</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Medical Database (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Psychology Database (Alumni)</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection</collection><collection>Hospital Premium Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016)</collection><collection>Research Library (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central UK/Ireland</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Essentials</collection><collection>ProQuest Central</collection><collection>Environmental Sciences and Pollution Management</collection><collection>ProQuest One Community College</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Korea</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection</collection><collection>Health Research Premium Collection (Alumni)</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Student</collection><collection>Research Library Prep</collection><collection>ProQuest Health & Medical Complete (Alumni)</collection><collection>Health & Medical Collection (Alumni Edition)</collection><collection>Medical Database</collection><collection>Psychology Database</collection><collection>Research Library</collection><collection>Research Library (Corporate)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE)</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic</collection><collection>ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition</collection><collection>ProQuest One Psychology</collection><collection>ProQuest Central Basic</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Animal cognition</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Shearer, David J.</au><au>Beilke, Elizabeth A.</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Playing it by ear: gregarious sparrows recognize and respond to isolated wingbeat sounds and predator-based cues</atitle><jtitle>Animal cognition</jtitle><stitle>Anim Cogn</stitle><addtitle>Anim Cogn</addtitle><date>2023-03-01</date><risdate>2023</risdate><volume>26</volume><issue>2</issue><spage>357</spage><epage>367</epage><pages>357-367</pages><issn>1435-9448</issn><eissn>1435-9456</eissn><abstract>The ability to detect an incoming attack provides a final opportunity for an animal to avoid predation. In birds, vision is the main sensory mode in detecting attacks, but auditory cues likely play an important role. The role of auditory cues from predators themselves remains largely unstudied. We evaluated the ability of free-living, gregarious sparrows (Passerellidae) to recognize attacks based on the non-vocal sounds made by predators or indirect auditory cues of ongoing attacks, mainly in the form of brief wingbeat sequences from predatory and non-predatory birds. Behavioral responses to playbacks were video-recorded and expressed in terms of a flock’s propensity to respond, either by flushing to cover, becoming vigilant, or both. Sparrows responded equally to hawk wingbeats and those of small passerines. Both predator and non-predator wingbeat sequences induced anti-predator responses, especially when played loudly. Loud control sounds, such as hammering, induced few responses. Birds also responded to the sounds of a walking and running terrestrial predator (a dog), but reactions to the walking predator often involved birds jumping onto objects for a better view of their surroundings rather than immediate flight to cover. In an additional experiment, we examined how characteristics of wingbeat sequences (i.e., the number and cadence of hawk wingbeats) affected passerine responses. It indicated that only two consecutive hawk wingbeats, presented at a natural cadence, are necessary to elicit a strong response to a playback. Single hawk wingbeats induced only weak escape responses, as did artificially slowed cadences. Birds in general likely possess the ability to recognize non-vocal, auditory cues of incoming attacks, which may be produced by approaching threats or departing congeners.</abstract><cop>Berlin/Heidelberg</cop><pub>Springer Berlin Heidelberg</pub><pmid>35930165</pmid><doi>10.1007/s10071-022-01663-z</doi><tpages>11</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7364-7521</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3941-8722</orcidid></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 1435-9448 |
ispartof | Animal cognition, 2023-03, Vol.26 (2), p.357-367 |
issn | 1435-9448 1435-9456 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2699704831 |
source | MEDLINE; SpringerNature Journals |
subjects | Animal cognition Animals Anti-predator behavior Behavior Behavioral Sciences Biomedical and Life Sciences Birds Birds of prey Congeners Cues Dogs Escape behavior Experiments Hearing Life Sciences Original Paper Playback Playbacks Predation Predators Predatory Behavior - physiology Psychology Research Sequences Sparrows Walking Zoology |
title | Playing it by ear: gregarious sparrows recognize and respond to isolated wingbeat sounds and predator-based cues |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-01-01T01%3A05%3A31IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Playing%20it%20by%20ear:%20gregarious%20sparrows%20recognize%20and%20respond%20to%20isolated%20wingbeat%20sounds%20and%20predator-based%20cues&rft.jtitle=Animal%20cognition&rft.au=Shearer,%20David%20J.&rft.date=2023-03-01&rft.volume=26&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=357&rft.epage=367&rft.pages=357-367&rft.issn=1435-9448&rft.eissn=1435-9456&rft_id=info:doi/10.1007/s10071-022-01663-z&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2699704831%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2779257974&rft_id=info:pmid/35930165&rfr_iscdi=true |