Online representations of non-canonical sentences are more than good-enough

Proponents of good-enough processing suggest that readers often (mis)interpret certain sentences using fast-and-frugal heuristics, such that for non-canonical sentences (e.g., The dog was bitten by the man) people confuse the thematic roles of the nouns. We tested this theory by examining the effect...

Ausführliche Beschreibung

Gespeichert in:
Bibliographische Detailangaben
Veröffentlicht in:Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006) 2022-01, Vol.75 (1), p.30-42
Hauptverfasser: Cutter, Michael G, Paterson, Kevin B, Filik, Ruth
Format: Artikel
Sprache:eng
Schlagworte:
Online-Zugang:Volltext
Tags: Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
container_end_page 42
container_issue 1
container_start_page 30
container_title Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)
container_volume 75
creator Cutter, Michael G
Paterson, Kevin B
Filik, Ruth
description Proponents of good-enough processing suggest that readers often (mis)interpret certain sentences using fast-and-frugal heuristics, such that for non-canonical sentences (e.g., The dog was bitten by the man) people confuse the thematic roles of the nouns. We tested this theory by examining the effect of sentence canonicality on the reading of a follow-up sentence. In a self-paced reading study, 60 young and 60 older adults read an implausible sentence in either canonical (e.g., It was the peasant that executed the king) or non-canonical form (e.g., It was the king that was executed by the peasant), followed by a sentence that was implausible given a good-enough misinterpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the peasant rode back to the countryside) or a sentence that was implausible given a correct interpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the king rode back to his castle). We hypothesised that if non-canonical sentences are systematically misinterpreted, then sentence canonicality would differentially affect the reading of the two different follow-up types. Our data suggested that participants derived the same interpretations for canonical and non-canonical sentences, with no modulating effect of age group. Our findings suggest that readers do not derive an incorrect interpretation of non-canonical sentences during initial parsing, consistent with theories of misinterpretation effects that instead attribute these effects to post-interpretative processes.
doi_str_mv 10.1177/17470218211032043
format Article
fullrecord <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2546600859</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><sage_id>10.1177_17470218211032043</sage_id><sourcerecordid>2546600859</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c411t-dbb117e389b2ba7689a1dccde11ea8c657399a97b2db03ec65d0575b071587643</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp1kE1PwzAMhiMEYmPwA7igSly4dMRJ06RHNPElJu0C5ypNva1Tl4ykPfDvydgYEoiLbdmPX1svIZdAxwBS3oLMJGWgGADljGb8iAy3vZQylh8falADchbCikZC5vKUDHgGKhO5GJKXmW0bi4nHjceAttNd42xI3DyxzqZGx9gY3SbbGVqDIdEek7WLoVtqmyycq1O0rl8sz8nJXLcBL_Z5RN4e7l8nT-l09vg8uZumJgPo0rqq4vfIVVGxSstcFRpqY2oEQK1MLiQvCl3IitUV5RgbNRVSVFSCUDLP-Ijc7HQ33r33GLpy3QSDbastuj6UTGR5TqkSRUSvf6Er13sbv4tUoTJO6RcFO8p4F4LHebnxzVr7jxJouXW6_ON03LnaK_fVGuvDxre1ERjvgKAX-HP2f8VPN8iEkw</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2598430059</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Online representations of non-canonical sentences are more than good-enough</title><source>SAGE Complete A-Z List</source><source>MEDLINE</source><creator>Cutter, Michael G ; Paterson, Kevin B ; Filik, Ruth</creator><creatorcontrib>Cutter, Michael G ; Paterson, Kevin B ; Filik, Ruth</creatorcontrib><description>Proponents of good-enough processing suggest that readers often (mis)interpret certain sentences using fast-and-frugal heuristics, such that for non-canonical sentences (e.g., The dog was bitten by the man) people confuse the thematic roles of the nouns. We tested this theory by examining the effect of sentence canonicality on the reading of a follow-up sentence. In a self-paced reading study, 60 young and 60 older adults read an implausible sentence in either canonical (e.g., It was the peasant that executed the king) or non-canonical form (e.g., It was the king that was executed by the peasant), followed by a sentence that was implausible given a good-enough misinterpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the peasant rode back to the countryside) or a sentence that was implausible given a correct interpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the king rode back to his castle). We hypothesised that if non-canonical sentences are systematically misinterpreted, then sentence canonicality would differentially affect the reading of the two different follow-up types. Our data suggested that participants derived the same interpretations for canonical and non-canonical sentences, with no modulating effect of age group. Our findings suggest that readers do not derive an incorrect interpretation of non-canonical sentences during initial parsing, consistent with theories of misinterpretation effects that instead attribute these effects to post-interpretative processes.</description><identifier>ISSN: 1747-0218</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1747-0226</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1177/17470218211032043</identifier><identifier>PMID: 34184565</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>London, England: SAGE Publications</publisher><subject>Comprehension ; Heuristics ; Humans ; Language ; Reading</subject><ispartof>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006), 2022-01, Vol.75 (1), p.30-42</ispartof><rights>Experimental Psychology Society 2021</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c411t-dbb117e389b2ba7689a1dccde11ea8c657399a97b2db03ec65d0575b071587643</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c411t-dbb117e389b2ba7689a1dccde11ea8c657399a97b2db03ec65d0575b071587643</cites><orcidid>0000-0002-8150-8628 ; 0000-0001-9022-8364 ; 0000-0002-3649-1142</orcidid></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktopdf>$$Uhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/17470218211032043$$EPDF$$P50$$Gsage$$H</linktopdf><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/17470218211032043$$EHTML$$P50$$Gsage$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,776,780,21798,27901,27902,43597,43598</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/34184565$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Cutter, Michael G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Paterson, Kevin B</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Filik, Ruth</creatorcontrib><title>Online representations of non-canonical sentences are more than good-enough</title><title>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)</title><addtitle>Q J Exp Psychol (Hove)</addtitle><description>Proponents of good-enough processing suggest that readers often (mis)interpret certain sentences using fast-and-frugal heuristics, such that for non-canonical sentences (e.g., The dog was bitten by the man) people confuse the thematic roles of the nouns. We tested this theory by examining the effect of sentence canonicality on the reading of a follow-up sentence. In a self-paced reading study, 60 young and 60 older adults read an implausible sentence in either canonical (e.g., It was the peasant that executed the king) or non-canonical form (e.g., It was the king that was executed by the peasant), followed by a sentence that was implausible given a good-enough misinterpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the peasant rode back to the countryside) or a sentence that was implausible given a correct interpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the king rode back to his castle). We hypothesised that if non-canonical sentences are systematically misinterpreted, then sentence canonicality would differentially affect the reading of the two different follow-up types. Our data suggested that participants derived the same interpretations for canonical and non-canonical sentences, with no modulating effect of age group. Our findings suggest that readers do not derive an incorrect interpretation of non-canonical sentences during initial parsing, consistent with theories of misinterpretation effects that instead attribute these effects to post-interpretative processes.</description><subject>Comprehension</subject><subject>Heuristics</subject><subject>Humans</subject><subject>Language</subject><subject>Reading</subject><issn>1747-0218</issn><issn>1747-0226</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2022</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><sourceid>EIF</sourceid><recordid>eNp1kE1PwzAMhiMEYmPwA7igSly4dMRJ06RHNPElJu0C5ypNva1Tl4ykPfDvydgYEoiLbdmPX1svIZdAxwBS3oLMJGWgGADljGb8iAy3vZQylh8falADchbCikZC5vKUDHgGKhO5GJKXmW0bi4nHjceAttNd42xI3DyxzqZGx9gY3SbbGVqDIdEek7WLoVtqmyycq1O0rl8sz8nJXLcBL_Z5RN4e7l8nT-l09vg8uZumJgPo0rqq4vfIVVGxSstcFRpqY2oEQK1MLiQvCl3IitUV5RgbNRVSVFSCUDLP-Ijc7HQ33r33GLpy3QSDbastuj6UTGR5TqkSRUSvf6Er13sbv4tUoTJO6RcFO8p4F4LHebnxzVr7jxJouXW6_ON03LnaK_fVGuvDxre1ERjvgKAX-HP2f8VPN8iEkw</recordid><startdate>202201</startdate><enddate>202201</enddate><creator>Cutter, Michael G</creator><creator>Paterson, Kevin B</creator><creator>Filik, Ruth</creator><general>SAGE Publications</general><general>Sage Publications Ltd</general><scope>CGR</scope><scope>CUY</scope><scope>CVF</scope><scope>ECM</scope><scope>EIF</scope><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8150-8628</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9022-8364</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3649-1142</orcidid></search><sort><creationdate>202201</creationdate><title>Online representations of non-canonical sentences are more than good-enough</title><author>Cutter, Michael G ; Paterson, Kevin B ; Filik, Ruth</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c411t-dbb117e389b2ba7689a1dccde11ea8c657399a97b2db03ec65d0575b071587643</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2022</creationdate><topic>Comprehension</topic><topic>Heuristics</topic><topic>Humans</topic><topic>Language</topic><topic>Reading</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Cutter, Michael G</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Paterson, Kevin B</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Filik, Ruth</creatorcontrib><collection>Medline</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE (Ovid)</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>MEDLINE</collection><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Cutter, Michael G</au><au>Paterson, Kevin B</au><au>Filik, Ruth</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Online representations of non-canonical sentences are more than good-enough</atitle><jtitle>Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)</jtitle><addtitle>Q J Exp Psychol (Hove)</addtitle><date>2022-01</date><risdate>2022</risdate><volume>75</volume><issue>1</issue><spage>30</spage><epage>42</epage><pages>30-42</pages><issn>1747-0218</issn><eissn>1747-0226</eissn><abstract>Proponents of good-enough processing suggest that readers often (mis)interpret certain sentences using fast-and-frugal heuristics, such that for non-canonical sentences (e.g., The dog was bitten by the man) people confuse the thematic roles of the nouns. We tested this theory by examining the effect of sentence canonicality on the reading of a follow-up sentence. In a self-paced reading study, 60 young and 60 older adults read an implausible sentence in either canonical (e.g., It was the peasant that executed the king) or non-canonical form (e.g., It was the king that was executed by the peasant), followed by a sentence that was implausible given a good-enough misinterpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the peasant rode back to the countryside) or a sentence that was implausible given a correct interpretation of the first sentence (e.g., Afterwards, the king rode back to his castle). We hypothesised that if non-canonical sentences are systematically misinterpreted, then sentence canonicality would differentially affect the reading of the two different follow-up types. Our data suggested that participants derived the same interpretations for canonical and non-canonical sentences, with no modulating effect of age group. Our findings suggest that readers do not derive an incorrect interpretation of non-canonical sentences during initial parsing, consistent with theories of misinterpretation effects that instead attribute these effects to post-interpretative processes.</abstract><cop>London, England</cop><pub>SAGE Publications</pub><pmid>34184565</pmid><doi>10.1177/17470218211032043</doi><tpages>13</tpages><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8150-8628</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9022-8364</orcidid><orcidid>https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3649-1142</orcidid><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record>
fulltext fulltext
identifier ISSN: 1747-0218
ispartof Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006), 2022-01, Vol.75 (1), p.30-42
issn 1747-0218
1747-0226
language eng
recordid cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2546600859
source SAGE Complete A-Z List; MEDLINE
subjects Comprehension
Heuristics
Humans
Language
Reading
title Online representations of non-canonical sentences are more than good-enough
url https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2025-02-01T21%3A47%3A33IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Online%20representations%20of%20non-canonical%20sentences%20are%20more%20than%20good-enough&rft.jtitle=Quarterly%20journal%20of%20experimental%20psychology%20(2006)&rft.au=Cutter,%20Michael%20G&rft.date=2022-01&rft.volume=75&rft.issue=1&rft.spage=30&rft.epage=42&rft.pages=30-42&rft.issn=1747-0218&rft.eissn=1747-0226&rft_id=info:doi/10.1177/17470218211032043&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2546600859%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2598430059&rft_id=info:pmid/34184565&rft_sage_id=10.1177_17470218211032043&rfr_iscdi=true