Pause for effect: A 10-s interviewer wait time gives children time to respond to open-ended prompts
•During interviews about an event, children often took more than 5 s to respond.•Most pauses in children's narratives that were followed by event information were less than 10 s long.•Pauses in ongoing narratives were longer among older, compared to younger, children.•Interviewers usually compl...
Gespeichert in:
Veröffentlicht in: | Journal of experimental child psychology 2020-06, Vol.194, p.104824-104824, Article 104824 |
---|---|
Hauptverfasser: | , , , |
Format: | Artikel |
Sprache: | eng |
Schlagworte: | |
Online-Zugang: | Volltext |
Tags: |
Tag hinzufügen
Keine Tags, Fügen Sie den ersten Tag hinzu!
|
container_end_page | 104824 |
---|---|
container_issue | |
container_start_page | 104824 |
container_title | Journal of experimental child psychology |
container_volume | 194 |
creator | Rezmer, Brooke E. Trager, Lisa A. Catlin, Mary Poole, Debra Ann |
description | •During interviews about an event, children often took more than 5 s to respond.•Most pauses in children's narratives that were followed by event information were less than 10 s long.•Pauses in ongoing narratives were longer among older, compared to younger, children.•Interviewers usually complied with a 10-s wait time rule.•A 10-s interviewer wait time is a practical guideline that gives children time to talk.
When researchers and helping professionals interview children about a target event, how long should they tolerate silence before delivering another prompt? In other words, at what point are children so unlikely to begin talking again that continued silence would likely be unproductive? To test the reasonableness of a 10-s wait time guideline during open-ended prompting, we analyzed the wait times of research assistants (N = 7) who interviewed with a 10-s guideline, timed how quickly children responded to prompts, and also timed pauses within children’s event narratives. In our sample (105 conversations with children aged 4–8 years), interviewers complied fully with the 10-s rule in the majority of interviews, children often paused for longer than 5 s before beginning to talk about the event or continuing a narrative, and more than 96% of children’s pauses that were followed by event information fell within the 10-s window. These findings show that the 10-s wait time was a practical guideline that gave children time to respond without peppering interviews with uncomfortably long pauses. We conclude that adding wait time guidelines to protocols for interviewing children, and augmenting guidelines with wait time training for research assistants and helping professionals, could improve the quality of information obtained from children and advance our understanding of age differences in event memory. |
doi_str_mv | 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104824 |
format | Article |
fullrecord | <record><control><sourceid>proquest_cross</sourceid><recordid>TN_cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2371141512</recordid><sourceformat>XML</sourceformat><sourcesystem>PC</sourcesystem><els_id>S0022096519305909</els_id><sourcerecordid>2371141512</sourcerecordid><originalsourceid>FETCH-LOGICAL-c400t-e71351d84fca3078061ca831c04b4c1329873ea546e7f93e6ec3391ed62af163</originalsourceid><addsrcrecordid>eNp9kMtOwzAQRS0EoqXwAyyQl2xSPLbzQmyqipdUCRbdW649AVfNAztpxd-TKIUlq5m5unNHcwi5BjYHBsnddr5F08w544MgMy5PyBRYnkRMxukpmTLGedTP8YRchLBlDCCR4pxMBAeeQi6mxLzrLiAtak-xKNC093RBgUWBuqpFv3d4QE8P2rW0dSXSD7fHQM2n21mP1ai1NfUYmrqyQ1s3WEVYWbS08XXZtOGSnBV6F_DqWGdk_fS4Xr5Eq7fn1-ViFRnJWBthCiIGm8nCaMHSjCVgdCbAMLmRBgTPs1SgjmWCaZELTNAIkQPahOsCEjEjt2Nsf_arw9Cq0gWDu52usO6C4iIFkBAD7618tBpfh-CxUI13pfbfCpga2KqtGtiqga0a2fZLN8f8blOi_Vv5hdkbHkYD9k_25LwKxmFl0Drfk1W2dv_l_wB0u4lZ</addsrcrecordid><sourcetype>Aggregation Database</sourcetype><iscdi>true</iscdi><recordtype>article</recordtype><pqid>2371141512</pqid></control><display><type>article</type><title>Pause for effect: A 10-s interviewer wait time gives children time to respond to open-ended prompts</title><source>Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier)</source><creator>Rezmer, Brooke E. ; Trager, Lisa A. ; Catlin, Mary ; Poole, Debra Ann</creator><creatorcontrib>Rezmer, Brooke E. ; Trager, Lisa A. ; Catlin, Mary ; Poole, Debra Ann</creatorcontrib><description>•During interviews about an event, children often took more than 5 s to respond.•Most pauses in children's narratives that were followed by event information were less than 10 s long.•Pauses in ongoing narratives were longer among older, compared to younger, children.•Interviewers usually complied with a 10-s wait time rule.•A 10-s interviewer wait time is a practical guideline that gives children time to talk.
When researchers and helping professionals interview children about a target event, how long should they tolerate silence before delivering another prompt? In other words, at what point are children so unlikely to begin talking again that continued silence would likely be unproductive? To test the reasonableness of a 10-s wait time guideline during open-ended prompting, we analyzed the wait times of research assistants (N = 7) who interviewed with a 10-s guideline, timed how quickly children responded to prompts, and also timed pauses within children’s event narratives. In our sample (105 conversations with children aged 4–8 years), interviewers complied fully with the 10-s rule in the majority of interviews, children often paused for longer than 5 s before beginning to talk about the event or continuing a narrative, and more than 96% of children’s pauses that were followed by event information fell within the 10-s window. These findings show that the 10-s wait time was a practical guideline that gave children time to respond without peppering interviews with uncomfortably long pauses. We conclude that adding wait time guidelines to protocols for interviewing children, and augmenting guidelines with wait time training for research assistants and helping professionals, could improve the quality of information obtained from children and advance our understanding of age differences in event memory.</description><identifier>ISSN: 0022-0965</identifier><identifier>EISSN: 1096-0457</identifier><identifier>DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104824</identifier><identifier>PMID: 32127193</identifier><language>eng</language><publisher>United States: Elsevier Inc</publisher><subject>Children ; Interviewer wait time ; Interviewing ; Interviewing guidelines ; Open-ended prompts ; Pause times</subject><ispartof>Journal of experimental child psychology, 2020-06, Vol.194, p.104824-104824, Article 104824</ispartof><rights>2020 Elsevier Inc.</rights><rights>Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.</rights><lds50>peer_reviewed</lds50><oa>free_for_read</oa><woscitedreferencessubscribed>false</woscitedreferencessubscribed><citedby>FETCH-LOGICAL-c400t-e71351d84fca3078061ca831c04b4c1329873ea546e7f93e6ec3391ed62af163</citedby><cites>FETCH-LOGICAL-c400t-e71351d84fca3078061ca831c04b4c1329873ea546e7f93e6ec3391ed62af163</cites></display><links><openurl>$$Topenurl_article</openurl><openurlfulltext>$$Topenurlfull_article</openurlfulltext><thumbnail>$$Tsyndetics_thumb_exl</thumbnail><linktohtml>$$Uhttps://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104824$$EHTML$$P50$$Gelsevier$$H</linktohtml><link.rule.ids>314,780,784,3550,27924,27925,45995</link.rule.ids><backlink>$$Uhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32127193$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed$$Hfree_for_read</backlink></links><search><creatorcontrib>Rezmer, Brooke E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Trager, Lisa A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Catlin, Mary</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Poole, Debra Ann</creatorcontrib><title>Pause for effect: A 10-s interviewer wait time gives children time to respond to open-ended prompts</title><title>Journal of experimental child psychology</title><addtitle>J Exp Child Psychol</addtitle><description>•During interviews about an event, children often took more than 5 s to respond.•Most pauses in children's narratives that were followed by event information were less than 10 s long.•Pauses in ongoing narratives were longer among older, compared to younger, children.•Interviewers usually complied with a 10-s wait time rule.•A 10-s interviewer wait time is a practical guideline that gives children time to talk.
When researchers and helping professionals interview children about a target event, how long should they tolerate silence before delivering another prompt? In other words, at what point are children so unlikely to begin talking again that continued silence would likely be unproductive? To test the reasonableness of a 10-s wait time guideline during open-ended prompting, we analyzed the wait times of research assistants (N = 7) who interviewed with a 10-s guideline, timed how quickly children responded to prompts, and also timed pauses within children’s event narratives. In our sample (105 conversations with children aged 4–8 years), interviewers complied fully with the 10-s rule in the majority of interviews, children often paused for longer than 5 s before beginning to talk about the event or continuing a narrative, and more than 96% of children’s pauses that were followed by event information fell within the 10-s window. These findings show that the 10-s wait time was a practical guideline that gave children time to respond without peppering interviews with uncomfortably long pauses. We conclude that adding wait time guidelines to protocols for interviewing children, and augmenting guidelines with wait time training for research assistants and helping professionals, could improve the quality of information obtained from children and advance our understanding of age differences in event memory.</description><subject>Children</subject><subject>Interviewer wait time</subject><subject>Interviewing</subject><subject>Interviewing guidelines</subject><subject>Open-ended prompts</subject><subject>Pause times</subject><issn>0022-0965</issn><issn>1096-0457</issn><fulltext>true</fulltext><rsrctype>article</rsrctype><creationdate>2020</creationdate><recordtype>article</recordtype><recordid>eNp9kMtOwzAQRS0EoqXwAyyQl2xSPLbzQmyqipdUCRbdW649AVfNAztpxd-TKIUlq5m5unNHcwi5BjYHBsnddr5F08w544MgMy5PyBRYnkRMxukpmTLGedTP8YRchLBlDCCR4pxMBAeeQi6mxLzrLiAtak-xKNC093RBgUWBuqpFv3d4QE8P2rW0dSXSD7fHQM2n21mP1ai1NfUYmrqyQ1s3WEVYWbS08XXZtOGSnBV6F_DqWGdk_fS4Xr5Eq7fn1-ViFRnJWBthCiIGm8nCaMHSjCVgdCbAMLmRBgTPs1SgjmWCaZELTNAIkQPahOsCEjEjt2Nsf_arw9Cq0gWDu52usO6C4iIFkBAD7618tBpfh-CxUI13pfbfCpga2KqtGtiqga0a2fZLN8f8blOi_Vv5hdkbHkYD9k_25LwKxmFl0Drfk1W2dv_l_wB0u4lZ</recordid><startdate>202006</startdate><enddate>202006</enddate><creator>Rezmer, Brooke E.</creator><creator>Trager, Lisa A.</creator><creator>Catlin, Mary</creator><creator>Poole, Debra Ann</creator><general>Elsevier Inc</general><scope>NPM</scope><scope>AAYXX</scope><scope>CITATION</scope><scope>7X8</scope></search><sort><creationdate>202006</creationdate><title>Pause for effect: A 10-s interviewer wait time gives children time to respond to open-ended prompts</title><author>Rezmer, Brooke E. ; Trager, Lisa A. ; Catlin, Mary ; Poole, Debra Ann</author></sort><facets><frbrtype>5</frbrtype><frbrgroupid>cdi_FETCH-LOGICAL-c400t-e71351d84fca3078061ca831c04b4c1329873ea546e7f93e6ec3391ed62af163</frbrgroupid><rsrctype>articles</rsrctype><prefilter>articles</prefilter><language>eng</language><creationdate>2020</creationdate><topic>Children</topic><topic>Interviewer wait time</topic><topic>Interviewing</topic><topic>Interviewing guidelines</topic><topic>Open-ended prompts</topic><topic>Pause times</topic><toplevel>peer_reviewed</toplevel><toplevel>online_resources</toplevel><creatorcontrib>Rezmer, Brooke E.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Trager, Lisa A.</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Catlin, Mary</creatorcontrib><creatorcontrib>Poole, Debra Ann</creatorcontrib><collection>PubMed</collection><collection>CrossRef</collection><collection>MEDLINE - Academic</collection><jtitle>Journal of experimental child psychology</jtitle></facets><delivery><delcategory>Remote Search Resource</delcategory><fulltext>fulltext</fulltext></delivery><addata><au>Rezmer, Brooke E.</au><au>Trager, Lisa A.</au><au>Catlin, Mary</au><au>Poole, Debra Ann</au><format>journal</format><genre>article</genre><ristype>JOUR</ristype><atitle>Pause for effect: A 10-s interviewer wait time gives children time to respond to open-ended prompts</atitle><jtitle>Journal of experimental child psychology</jtitle><addtitle>J Exp Child Psychol</addtitle><date>2020-06</date><risdate>2020</risdate><volume>194</volume><spage>104824</spage><epage>104824</epage><pages>104824-104824</pages><artnum>104824</artnum><issn>0022-0965</issn><eissn>1096-0457</eissn><abstract>•During interviews about an event, children often took more than 5 s to respond.•Most pauses in children's narratives that were followed by event information were less than 10 s long.•Pauses in ongoing narratives were longer among older, compared to younger, children.•Interviewers usually complied with a 10-s wait time rule.•A 10-s interviewer wait time is a practical guideline that gives children time to talk.
When researchers and helping professionals interview children about a target event, how long should they tolerate silence before delivering another prompt? In other words, at what point are children so unlikely to begin talking again that continued silence would likely be unproductive? To test the reasonableness of a 10-s wait time guideline during open-ended prompting, we analyzed the wait times of research assistants (N = 7) who interviewed with a 10-s guideline, timed how quickly children responded to prompts, and also timed pauses within children’s event narratives. In our sample (105 conversations with children aged 4–8 years), interviewers complied fully with the 10-s rule in the majority of interviews, children often paused for longer than 5 s before beginning to talk about the event or continuing a narrative, and more than 96% of children’s pauses that were followed by event information fell within the 10-s window. These findings show that the 10-s wait time was a practical guideline that gave children time to respond without peppering interviews with uncomfortably long pauses. We conclude that adding wait time guidelines to protocols for interviewing children, and augmenting guidelines with wait time training for research assistants and helping professionals, could improve the quality of information obtained from children and advance our understanding of age differences in event memory.</abstract><cop>United States</cop><pub>Elsevier Inc</pub><pmid>32127193</pmid><doi>10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104824</doi><tpages>1</tpages><oa>free_for_read</oa></addata></record> |
fulltext | fulltext |
identifier | ISSN: 0022-0965 |
ispartof | Journal of experimental child psychology, 2020-06, Vol.194, p.104824-104824, Article 104824 |
issn | 0022-0965 1096-0457 |
language | eng |
recordid | cdi_proquest_miscellaneous_2371141512 |
source | Access via ScienceDirect (Elsevier) |
subjects | Children Interviewer wait time Interviewing Interviewing guidelines Open-ended prompts Pause times |
title | Pause for effect: A 10-s interviewer wait time gives children time to respond to open-ended prompts |
url | https://sfx.bib-bvb.de/sfx_tum?ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info:ofi/enc:UTF-8&ctx_tim=2024-12-27T18%3A30%3A35IST&url_ver=Z39.88-2004&url_ctx_fmt=infofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&rfr_id=info:sid/primo.exlibrisgroup.com:primo3-Article-proquest_cross&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Pause%20for%20effect:%20A%2010-s%20interviewer%20wait%20time%20gives%20children%20time%20to%20respond%20to%20open-ended%20prompts&rft.jtitle=Journal%20of%20experimental%20child%20psychology&rft.au=Rezmer,%20Brooke%20E.&rft.date=2020-06&rft.volume=194&rft.spage=104824&rft.epage=104824&rft.pages=104824-104824&rft.artnum=104824&rft.issn=0022-0965&rft.eissn=1096-0457&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104824&rft_dat=%3Cproquest_cross%3E2371141512%3C/proquest_cross%3E%3Curl%3E%3C/url%3E&disable_directlink=true&sfx.directlink=off&sfx.report_link=0&rft_id=info:oai/&rft_pqid=2371141512&rft_id=info:pmid/32127193&rft_els_id=S0022096519305909&rfr_iscdi=true |